Spelling suggestions: "subject:"[een] BEHAVIOR"" "subject:"[enn] BEHAVIOR""
211 |
Response by waterfowl and blackbirds to an experimentally manipulated cattail marsh.Murkin, Henry Robert. January 1979 (has links)
No description available.
|
212 |
Investigating Environmentally Responsible Behavior: A Phenomenological Study of the Personal Behaviors of Acknowledged Leaders in the Area of Climate ChangeBraun, Nichole A. 29 August 2012 (has links)
No description available.
|
213 |
ZOO EXHIBIT DESIGN: A POST-OCCUPANCY EVALUATION AND COMPARISON OF ANIMAL ENCLOSURES.SHETTEL-NEUBER, MARY JOYCE. January 1986 (has links)
The present study, in contrast with previous work that has isolated one or two important factors influencing the status of the zoo, considered the three important zoo reference groups--animals, visitors, and staff members--and their interrelationships within the zoo environment. Two approaches were used to investigate the system of interactions within the zoo. First, an in-depth examination of a new set of naturalistic exhibits was performed. Second, a comparison of two of these naturalistic exhibits with two older, sterile exhibits which housed the same species at the same zoo was made. Multiple methods were used in the present study and included behavior mapping of visitors, staff, and animals, timing of visitor stays at exhibits, tracking of visitors through the exhibits, a visitor questionnaire, and interviews with staff members. One major finding was the lack of correspondence among the major groups as to the acceptability of exhibits. For example, one exhibit which was considered beneficial to the enclosed animals and was well utilized and positively evaluated by visitors presented staff members with great difficulties in animal containment and exhibit maintenance. Comparisons of naturalistic enclosures and sterile cement enclosures housing the same species revealed no consistent, clear-cut differences in animal and visitor behavior, however, attitudinal differences were found for staff members and visitors. Visitors and staff members preferred the naturalistic exhibits and perceived them as more beneficial to animals and visitors. These findings were discussed in terms of theoretical and applied issues relevant to zoo design and management and to research in zoos.
|
214 |
Tool use, foraging ecology and social dynamics in New Caledonian crowsBluff, Lucas January 2008 (has links)
No description available.
|
215 |
Cognitive psychology of tool use in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides)Weir, Alexander Allan Scott January 2005 (has links)
No description available.
|
216 |
Play and social relationships in the meerkat (Suricata suricatta)Sharpe, Lynda L. 04 1900 (has links)
Thesis (PhD)--Stellenbosch University, 2005. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: Despite more than three decades of research, and the postulation of more than 30 hypotheses
of function, the adaptive significance of play remains unknown. This study quantitatively
evaluated a selection of hypotheses of function, using data collected from a wild population
of small, social carnivore, the meerkat, Suricata suricatta. The study found that although play
in meerkats carried an energetic cost, with individuals modulating their frequency of play in
response to their energy intake, none of the hypotheses evaluated by the study could identify
the adaptive benefits that meerkats derived from play. Play did not increase 'social harmony'
by reducing aggression between playmates, nor did it strengthen an individual's bonds to its
social group, such that it remained in the group for longer, or contributed more to the group's
cooperative activities. There was no evidence that meerkats used play to strengthen alliances
between individuals, and young meerkats played no more frequently with their future
dispersal partners than with matched controls with which they did not disperse. Play fighting
experience did not improve a meerkat's subsequent fighting skills, and individuals that
ultimately won the dominant breeding position within a group (through serious fighting)
played no more frequently, and no more successfully, as youngsters, than the littermates that
they defeated in combat. Although play was inhibited by aggression, meerkats did not use
play to contest, assert or establish dominance status, and there was little evidence to suggest
that the preference young meerkats showed for play partners that were well matched in age,
size and ability arose from their use of play for self-assessment.
This study assessed only those hypotheses of function that predicted benefits that were of
importance to the inclusive fitness of the study species. For example, the enhancement of
social harmony and group cohesion should be invaluable to a species whose survival is
dependent upon social cooperation; and the high reproductive skew exhibited by this species
places huge value upon fighting skill and the ability to win social dominance. As a
consequence, this study's negative findings suggest strongly that play is not capable of
providing these benefits, and that play behaviour is unlikely to be used for these purposes in
any mammal species. I conclude that the most likely function of play (based on play's
ubiquitous characteristics, and the findings of neurological research on rats) is the promotion
of growth of the cerebral cortex. / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Ten spyte van meer as drie dekades van navorsing en die voorstelling van meer as 30
hipoteses oor funksie, bly die aanpassingswaarde van spelonbekend. Hierdie studie is 'n
kwantitatiewe evaluasie van verskeie hipoteses oor funksie, en gebruik data versamel vanuit
'n wilde bevolking van 'n klein sosiale karnivoor, die meerkat, Suricata suricatta. Die studie
het bevind dat hoewel spel in meerkaaie 'n energetiese koste beloop, met individue wat hul
spelfrekwensie aanpas by energie-inname, geen-een van die hipoteses onder beskouing die
aanpassingswaarde van spel vir meerkaaie kon verduidelik nie. Spel het nie "sosiale
harmonie" bevorder deur die afuame in aggressie tussen speelmaats nie, en het ook nie 'n
individu se verbintenis tot sy sosiale groep versterk sodat hy langer in die groep sou bly of
meer sou bydra tot samewerkingsaktiwiteite nie. Daar was geen bewyse vir die gebruik van
spel in die versterking van bondgenootskappe tussen individue nie, en jong meerkaaie het nie
meer gereeld met toekomstige verspreidings-venote gespeel as met gepaarde kontroles saam
met wie hulle nie uiteengegaan het nie. Speelse gevegte het nie 'n meerkat se daaropvolgende
gevegsvermoëns verbeter nie, en die individue wat uiteindelik die dominante
voortplantingsposisie in 'n groep gewen het (deur ernstige stryd) het nie meer gereeld ofmeer
suksesvol as jongelinge gespeel in vergelyking met die werpselmaats wat hulle in die stryd
oorwin het nie. Hoewel spel deur aggressie onderdruk is, het meerkaaie spel nie gebruik om
dominante range te beveg, bevestig of tot stand te bring nie. Daar was min bewyse ter
ondersteuning van die voorstel dat jong meerkaaie se voorkeur vir speelmaats wat hul gelyke
is in ouderdom, grootte en vermoë, onstaan het in die gebruik van spel vir selfondersoek.
Hierdie studie het slegs die hipoteses van funksie beskou wat voorspellings gemaak het
oor die voordele wat belangrik is in die inklusiewe fiksheid van die studie-species.
Byvoorbeeld, die verbetering van sosiale harmonie en groepsamehang behoort van
onskatbare waarde te wees vir 'n species wat afhanklik is van sosiale samewerking vir
oorlewing; en die hoë graad van voorkeuraanwas duidelik in hierdie species plaas groot
waarde op gevegsvaardighede en die vermoë om sosiale dominansie te wen. Gevolglik dui
hierdie ondersoek se negatiewe bevindinge daarop dat spel nie hierdie voordele kan bied nie,
en dat speelgedrag heel waarskynlik nie vir hierdie doeleindes in enige soogdier-species
gebruik word nie. Ek kom tot die gevolgtrekking dat die heel waarskynlikste funksie van spel
(gebaseer op spel se alomteenwoordige kenmerke en die bevindinge van neurologiese
navorsing op rotte) die bevordering van groei in die serebrale korteks is.
|
217 |
Spatial and social influences on the behaviour of captive chimpanzeesDuncan, Luke Mangaliso 05 September 2012 (has links)
Captive animals are frequently subject to imposed, uncontrollable stressors to which they
respond through behavioural flexibility, or, failing which, exhibit pathology. Chimpanzees
provide an intriguing model to examine how captive environments influence the responses of
animals to stress. My study investigates the responses of a group of chimpanzees to imposed
stressors of captivity at the Johannesburg Zoo, South Africa. My study comprised four
components. Firstly, I examined the effects of spatial restriction on chimpanzee behaviour
with regard to an enclosure enlargement, testing several existing models of coping with
spatial crowding and another model, based on the coping hypothesis of abnormal behaviour.
Behavioural observations of the chimpanzees in their indoor and outdoor exhibits before,
during, immediately after and 10 weeks after the enclosure reconstruction revealed that the
chimpanzees used tension-reduction and conflict-avoidance tactics as a means to cope with
spatial crowding. Moreover, abnormal behaviour appears to provide an outlet for stress under
crowding. Secondly, I assessed the long-term effects of past spatial environments on the
space use and group spacing of the chimpanzees, five years after the enclosure change.
Through behavioural observations and mapping the locations of individuals, I found that the
chimpanzees exhibit space-use bias and limited group spacing, contingent on the dimensions
of the old enclosure that were not explained by factors such as social or thermal conditions
and zoo visitor effects. I propose that the spacing patterns may be due to spatial learned
helplessness. Thirdly, I examined the effect of two social manipulations, mandated by zoo
management, on the behaviour and socio-dynamics of the chimpanzees. The chimpanzees
responded to social change through selective social interactions and non-social behavioural
responses suggest that removing an individual was less stressful than the merging of two
groups. Finally, I investigated the role of shade as a thermoregulatory resource for captive
chimpanzees. Individuals used shade frequently despite observations taking place during the
austral winter period, suggesting that shade is a valuable thermal resource for chimpanzees.
In conclusion, the chimpanzees responded to most imposed stressors (spatial crowding, social
change and thermal stress) through behavioural flexibility, implying successful coping, but
failed to cope with previous spatial restrictions, resulting in limited space-use behaviour.
|
218 |
Forgiveness and Health: Assessing the Mediating Effect of Health Behavior, Social Support, and Interpersonal FunctioningWebb, Jon R., Hirsch, Jameson K., Visser, Preston L., Brewer, Kenneth G. 01 September 2013 (has links)
Religiousness and spirituality are important to most Americans and while beneficial associations between forgiveness and health are consistently observed, little is known regarding the mechanism of association. Cross-sectional multiple mediation-based analyses of associations between dimensions of forgiveness and physical and mental health were conducted using a sample of 363 undergraduate students from rural Southern Appalachia. Controlling for demographic variables (i.e., gender, age, education, ethnicity, and marital status) and lifetime religiousness, multivariable analyses reflected associations of forgiveness of self and forgiveness of others, but not feeling forgiven by God, with physical health status, somatic symptoms, mental health status, and psychological distress. All such associations operated through health behavior and/or social support; however, only in the context of forgiveness of self did such associations also operate through interpersonal functioning (problems). While forgiveness of self and forgiveness of others each appear to have a robust indirect relationship with health, mediation-based associations involving forgiveness of self were nearly twice as frequent. It may be that forgiveness of self is relatively more important to health-related outcomes.
|
219 |
The Relationship between Psychological Contract Fulfillment and Employee Behavior:The Moderating Effect of PersonalityHuang, Chen-ying 21 August 2006 (has links)
In the traditional employment relationship, contract is an essential element to sustain the mutual relationship between employer and employee. In recent years, with the changes of business environment brought forward by the increasing globalization of business and by the dramatic rise in the numbers of mergers and restructurings, there has been a transformation of employment relationship. Psychological contract, a special type of contract, has become increasingly important under such dynamic environment.
After reviewing previous academic researches, we found that the issue about the concept of psychological contract was found in relation with employees¡¦ behavior. Based on the previous opinion, this study expects to prove that psychological contract fulfillment is strongly related to employees¡¦ behavior. We also discuss the moderating effect of personality on the relationship between psychological contract fulfillment and employees¡¦ behavior. We therefore intend to use questionnaire survey and using Taiwanese corporate with intention to prove the hypothesis of this paper.
The major finding of this study are as following¡G
1¡BDifferent types of psychological contracts fulfillment
are positively related to employees¡¦in-role behavior.
2¡BDifferent types of psychological contracts fulfillment
have different impact on organizational citizenship
behaviors directed at other individuals within the
organization.
3¡BPersonality has no moderating effect on the relationship
between psychological contract fulfillment and
employees¡¦ behavior.
4¡BPersonality has strong moderating effect on the
relationship between psychological contract fulfillment
and organizational citizenship behaviors directed at the
organization.
|
220 |
The effect of group size on feeding competition in blue gouramis (Pisces:Trichogaster trichopterus) /Syarifuddin, S. January 1993 (has links)
Laboratory experiments examined now aggressive behavior, use of space, activity and breathing frequency were affected by the number of fish competing for a concentrated, renewing, intermittent food source. Total aggression rate per fish and the proportion of long chases decreased in larger groups and increased during short periods in which food was unavailable. The proportional use of the area near the food source was maximal at intermediate group sizes suggesting that use of the feeding area was reduced by aggressive defense at low densities and by passive interference at high densities. Air-breathing frequency tended to be lower when food was available than when it was not, suggesting that gouramis reduce their use of atmospheric oxygen when it may result in loss of food to competitors. Group size affects the pattern of food competition more through economic resource defense than through passive interference.
|
Page generated in 0.0599 seconds