• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 89
  • 17
  • 8
  • 7
  • 6
  • 4
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 169
  • 124
  • 43
  • 41
  • 25
  • 18
  • 18
  • 15
  • 15
  • 14
  • 14
  • 13
  • 12
  • 11
  • 11
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Experiments in the control of binocular rivalry

Lack, Leon C. January 1972 (has links)
vi, 380 leaves : ill. / Title page, contents and abstract only. The complete thesis in print form is available from the University Library. / Thesis (Ph.D.1973) from the Dept. of Psychology, University of Adelaide
2

Experiments in the control of binocular rivalry.

Lack, Leon Colburn. January 1972 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (Ph.D. 1973) from the Dept. of Psychology, University of Adelaide.
3

Binocular vision: the relation of fusion to retinal rivalry

Humphriss, Deryck 14 August 2013 (has links)
Thesis (M.Sc.)--University of the Witwatersrand, 1961.
4

Binocular alignment and vergence errors in free space

Cornell Elaine. January 2004 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, University of Sydney, 2004. / Bibliography: leaves 113-120. Also available in print form.
5

Binocular alignment and vergence errors in free space

Cornell, Elaine January 2004 (has links)
Doctor of Philosophy / The human, along with other primates, has forward placed eyes, and an area of acute vision (the fovea) on each retina. The overlap of the visual fields and the hemi-decussation of the visual pathways at the optic chiasm provide the basis for binocular vision, in particular stereopsis, the accurate perception of the position of objects in three dimensional space and an improved ability to perceive the form of solid objects. An intricate system of eye movements is needed to achieve and maintain stable foveal fixation on each eye in an environment where visual targets vary in direction and depth, where the visual environment may be moving, the eyes or the rest of the body is moving. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the accuracy of binocular alignment for far and near fixations, under relatively natural conditions. To achieve binocular fixation, accurate vergence eye movements are required to align the eyes, and to maintain this alignment when a person changes fixation to objects situated at different distances from the eyes. ‘Pure’ vergence eye movements occur when these objects are situated along the mid sagittal plane, however, in natural conditions other eye movement systems are also involved. To understand the contribution of different eye movement systems to binocular fixation at different distances, the accuracy of binocular alignment in subjects with normal binocular single vision was evaluated in subjects with normal binocular vision under the following conditions • Fixation on targets along the mid sagittal plane (vergence eye movements only) • Fixation on targets displaced to either side of the mid sagittal plane (combined vergence eye movements and saccades • Fixation on earth fixed targets situated straight ahead in space, but with the head tilted to either side (combined vergence eye movements, saccades and torsional eye movements). The protocol for all experiments was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney and followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Throughout this thesis the term ‘binocular alignment’ will be used to describe the position of each eye during or following a change in vergence. The term ‘vergence error’ will refer to situations where the angle of vergence alignment is different from that required, so that the image of the fixation target does not fall on the fovea of one or both eyes.
6

Binocular alignment and vergence errors in free space

Cornell, Elaine January 2004 (has links)
Doctor of Philosophy / The human, along with other primates, has forward placed eyes, and an area of acute vision (the fovea) on each retina. The overlap of the visual fields and the hemi-decussation of the visual pathways at the optic chiasm provide the basis for binocular vision, in particular stereopsis, the accurate perception of the position of objects in three dimensional space and an improved ability to perceive the form of solid objects. An intricate system of eye movements is needed to achieve and maintain stable foveal fixation on each eye in an environment where visual targets vary in direction and depth, where the visual environment may be moving, the eyes or the rest of the body is moving. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the accuracy of binocular alignment for far and near fixations, under relatively natural conditions. To achieve binocular fixation, accurate vergence eye movements are required to align the eyes, and to maintain this alignment when a person changes fixation to objects situated at different distances from the eyes. ‘Pure’ vergence eye movements occur when these objects are situated along the mid sagittal plane, however, in natural conditions other eye movement systems are also involved. To understand the contribution of different eye movement systems to binocular fixation at different distances, the accuracy of binocular alignment in subjects with normal binocular single vision was evaluated in subjects with normal binocular vision under the following conditions • Fixation on targets along the mid sagittal plane (vergence eye movements only) • Fixation on targets displaced to either side of the mid sagittal plane (combined vergence eye movements and saccades • Fixation on earth fixed targets situated straight ahead in space, but with the head tilted to either side (combined vergence eye movements, saccades and torsional eye movements). The protocol for all experiments was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney and followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Throughout this thesis the term ‘binocular alignment’ will be used to describe the position of each eye during or following a change in vergence. The term ‘vergence error’ will refer to situations where the angle of vergence alignment is different from that required, so that the image of the fixation target does not fall on the fovea of one or both eyes.
7

Binocular vision skills in human observers /

Ajzenman, Heather. January 2008 (has links) (PDF)
Undergraduate honors paper--Mount Holyoke College, 2008. Program in Neuroscience and Behavior. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 87-89).
8

Contrast sensitivity as an indicator of binocular function

Tunnacliffe, A. H. January 1986 (has links)
No description available.
9

Vergence eye movements and stereopsis

Christophers, R. A. January 1996 (has links)
No description available.
10

Interocular interactions in normal and amblyopic visual systems

Vedamurthy, Indu, Optometry & Vision Science, Faculty of Science, UNSW January 2006 (has links)
The aim of this study was to add to our understanding of interocular interactions in normally sighted children (Group I, N=20), normal adults (Group II, N=20) and adults with anisometropic amblyopia (N=12) by investigating responses to a range of visual functions under three kinds of viewing condition. Visual functions tested were visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and alignment sensitivity. Stimuli were generated on a Cambridge VSG card driving a high resolution monitor and FE liquid crystal goggles, enabling three kinds of viewing conditions: 1. Monocular (non-tested eye occluded), used as a baseline for most functions. 2. Dichoptic (uniform field presented to the non-tested eye but with a binocular fusion lock). 3. Binocular. In general, binocular performance was better than monocular (binocular summation) but so too was dichoptic performance (dichoptic advantage). However there was much variation within individuals (the three functions showing different summation/advantage pattern) and between individuals. Significant conclusions include: (a) Maturational windows for interocular interactions differ for different spatial visual functions. (b) Interpretations of results from one visual function cannot be applied automatically to other functions. (c) Care must be taken in interpreting results based on 5 or fewer subjects.

Page generated in 0.0543 seconds