• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 294
  • 147
  • 36
  • 36
  • 36
  • 36
  • 36
  • 36
  • 16
  • 13
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 7
  • Tagged with
  • 620
  • 620
  • 144
  • 143
  • 143
  • 118
  • 78
  • 60
  • 53
  • 50
  • 44
  • 42
  • 42
  • 41
  • 41
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
31

A study of "r" articulatory proficiency as a function of speech sound discrimination skill

Mize, Carole Jean January 2011 (has links)
Digitized by Kansas Correctional Industries
32

Cross-spectral auditory grouping using the paradigm of rhythmic masking release

Turgeon, Martine. January 1999 (has links)
No description available.
33

Production and perceptions of VOT and high vowels by bilingual and monolingual speakers of Canadian English and Canadian French /

MacLeod, Andrea Asenath Nora. January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Washington, 2006. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 284-291).
34

Auditory object perception : counterpoint in a new context

Wright, James K. January 1986 (has links)
No description available.
35

Auditory ambience as an information display

King, Lisa Charmayne 05 1900 (has links)
No description available.
36

Aural preview for visually guided target acquisition and aiming

Elias, Bartholomew 08 1900 (has links)
No description available.
37

Cumulative effects in auditory stream segregation

Rogers, Wendy Laurel January 1991 (has links)
Nine experiments were done to test three theories of auditory stream segregation and to investigate some conditions under which segregated tones re-integrate. In two-part trials, subjects (adults with normal hearing) first heard a segregation-inducing "Induction Sequence" whose effects upon an immediately subsequent "Test Sequence" were measured. The Test Sequence always had tones that alternated rhythmically between two frequencies. Rhythm and total duration of Induction Sequence tones were varied in the first two studies. Similarity of Induction and Test Sequences aided segregation whereas rhythmic predictability and longer tone durations in the Induction Sequence did not. Frequency alternation during the Induction Sequence was not necessary to induce segregation in the Test Sequence. The effects of sudden and gradual changes in lateralization, spatial location and sound level were investigated also. The data suggest that explaining segregation by peripheral processes is inadequate and that, once a distinct percept emerges from an auditory scene, properties derived from the percept (particularly changes) are fed back to control the ongoing analysis of that scene. A neural adaptation to stimuli with constant properties may form part of this analysis.
38

The influence of log-frequency parallel gliding upon perceptual fusion

Turgeon, Martine January 1994 (has links)
It is generally recognized that simple harmonic ratios among partials promote their perceptual fusion. However, the influence of parallel gliding in log frequency upon fusion is not understood. The present experiment investigated fusion in relation to different types of parallel and non-parallel log-frequency motion of three concurrent tonal glides. The main hypothesis was that parallel motion on log-frequency-by-time coordinates favors fusion. It was reasoned that a higher degree of fusion of the glides evokes fewer auditory images. Fusion was thus measured by asking eighteen subjects to rate the number of distinct sounds perceived in various gliding stimuli. On test trials, subjects received a pair of stimuli in succession and had to judge which one contained more sounds and to rate the size of the difference on a 7-point scale. Each stimulus was a complex of three sinusoidal tones, gliding in frequency. Each 1400-ms three-glide complex was either increasing or decreasing in frequency, and the spacing among its components was either small, medium or large. The stimuli were aligned in one of five ways: (1) harmonically related, parallel, and therefore unequally spaced in log frequency, (2) inharmonic, parallel, and equally spaced in log frequency, (3) inharmonic, parallel, and unequally spaced in log frequency, (4) non-parallel and diverging in log frequency, and (5) non-parallel and converging in log frequency. Results showed that more sources were perceived under the three parallel conditions than under the two non-parallel ones (p $<$ 0.00001). Moreover, as the spacing between gliding partials increased, more distinct sounds were heard (p $<$ 0.00001). These results suggest that both spectral spacing and non-parallel log-frequency motion segregate concurrent glides. A multiple regression analysis showed that parallel log-frequency gliding promotes fusion over and above the contribution of average spectral spacing and harmonicity (p $<$.0001). The observed dat
39

How does the formation of an auditory unit affect the perception of a changing timbre?

Crum, Poppy. January 1999 (has links)
The effects of sub-unit formation on adult listeners' ability to notice changes in a continuously changing timbre (with fixed fundamental frequency) were studied. In Experiment 1, reaction times were recorded when listeners were asked to detect a change in the timbre of a 5-sec sound with spectral content that gradually shifted across time producing a perceptual change from "dull" to "bright." Sixteen silences were inserted in some conditions to cause the formation of short units. Listeners noticed the change sooner in these conditions than when the transitions were unbroken or broken by loud noise bursts. Experiments 2 and 3 looked at the individual unit and considered two separate processes as possible explanations for the previous results. Process 1 accentuates stimulus properties present at moments of onset and offset. Process 2, instead, uses onsets and offsets to signal the beginnings and ends of units and reduces the change perceived within a unit. The former would imply that listeners made onset comparisons in a point-to-point manner, whereas the latter would imply that the onsets, which formed units, enabled a unit-to-unit comparison. In Experiments 2 and 3 listeners were asked to distinguish between 1-sec sounds that both started with Timbre 1 and ended with Timbre 2, but were different in their internal rates of timbral change. Listener's performance was significantly poorer when asked to distinguish between two sounds that had identical onset and offset information in comparison to their performance when asked to distinguish either the first halves or second halves of these, which would differ in onset or offset information. Evidence suggests the presence of both Process 1 and Process 2.
40

Cross-spectral auditory grouping using the paradigm of rhythmic masking release

Turgeon, Martine. January 1999 (has links)
The cross-spectral grouping of complex sounds was investigated with the rhythmic masking release (RMR) paradigm. RMR involves the discrimination of one of two possible rhythms, despite perceptual masking of the rhythm by an irregular sequence of sounds identical to the rhythmic sounds that are interleaved among them. The rhythm can be released from masking by inducing the perceptual fusion of the irregular interfering sounds with concurrent "flanking" sounds situated in different frequency regions. The accuracy of the identified rhythm and its rated clarity in a two-alternative forced-choice procedure measured the degree of cross-spectral fusion of the interfering sounds with the flanking sounds. / The results obtained in six experiments suggest that temporal synchrony is sufficient for the fusion of (i) brief noise bursts that are widely apart in frequency (large DeltaF), that have uncorrelated high-rate intensity changes (HRIC), and that are spatially separated; and (ii) brief tones that are not harmonics of a common fundamental frequency (F0) and that are spatially-separated. An asynchrony of 20--40 ms is sufficient for the segregation of: (i) brief temporally-overlapping noise bursts with correlated HRIC and without spatial separations; and (ii) brief overlapping tones sharing a common F0 and without spatial separations. Intermediate asynchronies of 10--20 ms produce ambiguous cases of grouping and the presence of other segregation or fusion cues is critical to disambiguate them. / Thus, whereas uncorrelated ERIC, large DeltaF's, different F0's, and dichotic presentation all significantly affect the fusion of simultaneous or nearly simultaneous sounds, they are not sufficient by themselves to fully segregate these sounds; however, when a group of them act together, their synergetic action reinforces the effect of a small asynchrony in promoting segregation. When two sources are simultaneously active and emit short-duration sounds, separation of the sound sources in space has a negligible effect on their segregation. This research demonstrates the dominance of spectro-temporal cues (e.g., temporal synchrony) over spatial cues (e.g., common sound-source location) for the grouping of brief concurrent sounds. It also demonstrates that the auditory system is highly sensitive to any deviation from temporal synchrony. Further research is needed to establish whether the present conclusions apply to sounds of a longer duration.

Page generated in 0.0334 seconds