Spelling suggestions: "subject:"charts, diagrams, etc."" "subject:"dcharts, diagrams, etc.""
31 |
Exploring differences in teachers', administrators', and parents' preferences for data display and whether type of graphic display influences accuracy when extracting information /Alverson, Charlotte Y. January 2008 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Oregon, 2008. / Typescript. Includes vita and abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 147-151). Also available online in ProQuest, free to University of Oregon users.
|
32 |
The relationships between spatial ability, logical thinking, mathematics performance and kinematics graph interpretation skills of 12th grade physics studentsBektasli, Behzat, January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Ohio State University, 2006. / Title from first page of PDF file. Includes bibliographical references (p. 126-130).
|
33 |
Multivariate control charts for the mean vector and variance-covariance matrix with variable sampling intervalsCho, Gyo-Young 01 February 2006 (has links)
When using control charts to monitor a process it is frequently necessary to simultaneously monitor more than one parameter of the process. Multivariate control charts for monitoring the mean vector, for monitoring variance-covariance matrix and for simultaneously monitoring the mean vector and the variance-covariance matrix of a process with a multivariate normal distribution are investigated. A variable sampling interval (VSI) feature is considered in these charts.
Two basic approaches for using past sample information in the development of multivariate control charts are considered. The first approach, which is called the combine-accumulate approach, reduces each multivariate observation to a univariate statistic and then accumulates over past samples. The second approach, which is called the accumulate-combine approach, accumulates past sample information for each parameter and then forms a univariate statistic from the multivariate accumulations.
Multivariate control charts are compared on the basis of their average time to signal (ATS) performance. The numerical results show that the multivariate control charts based on the accumulate-combine approach are more efficient than the corresponding multivariate control charts based on the combine-accumulate approach in terms of ATS. Also VSI charts are more efficient than corresponding FSI charts. / Ph. D.
|
34 |
A synthesis of quality and process controlGraybeal, B. Cheree January 1986 (has links)
An improved quality control model is suggested in this thesis. The improved quality control model is derived by treating the quality control problem as a process control problem. The quality control model is developed by formulating a process control model in terms of product quality parameters and control variables which affect the product quality parameters. SQC is used in the model to provide estimates about the state of the product quality variable as the product is processed by the plant. A state variable approach is used to determine the optimal control strategy.
An example quality control model is formulated for a coke size-reduction process. Numerical values are assumed and sensitivity analysis results are discussed. The results show that the proposed quality control model is reasonable. Extensions to more complicated models are discussed. / M.S.
|
35 |
Variable sampling intervals for control charts using count dataShobe, Kristin N. January 1988 (has links)
This thesis examines the use of variable sampling intervals as they apply to control charts that use count data. Papers by Reynolds, Arnold, and R. Amin developed properties for charts with an underlying normal distribution. These properties are extended in this thesis to accommodate an underlying Poisson distribution. / Master of Science
|
36 |
Utilisation of the Road to Health Chart to improve the health of childrenMudau, Tshimangadzo Selina 11 1900 (has links)
The objectives of this study were to determine the nature of data recorded on the
Road to Health Card (RtHC) and its utilisation by nurses at primary health care
setting, comparing it with norms and standards on the RtHC guideline, and to provide
guidelines for optimal utilisation of the RtHC by health workers. A quantitative nonexperimental
descriptive method was used. Two methods were used to collect data;
document analysis of the RtHC and structure observation of nurses. A structured exit
interview of caregivers was conducted to validate observations from nurses. A
sample size of 18 nurses from all categories from six clinics was observed, and 36
RtHC of children under five years of age were analysed. Results indicated that data
recorded on the RtHC was mostly inaccurate, incomplete and not interpreted. The
study identified a need to train health workers on optimal utilisation of the RtHC
facilitating health improvement of children under five years of age. / Health Studies / M.A. (Health Studies)
|
37 |
Utilisation of the Road to Health Chart to improve the health of childrenMudau, Tshimangadzo Selina 11 1900 (has links)
The objectives of this study were to determine the nature of data recorded on the
Road to Health Card (RtHC) and its utilisation by nurses at primary health care
setting, comparing it with norms and standards on the RtHC guideline, and to provide
guidelines for optimal utilisation of the RtHC by health workers. A quantitative nonexperimental
descriptive method was used. Two methods were used to collect data;
document analysis of the RtHC and structure observation of nurses. A structured exit
interview of caregivers was conducted to validate observations from nurses. A
sample size of 18 nurses from all categories from six clinics was observed, and 36
RtHC of children under five years of age were analysed. Results indicated that data
recorded on the RtHC was mostly inaccurate, incomplete and not interpreted. The
study identified a need to train health workers on optimal utilisation of the RtHC
facilitating health improvement of children under five years of age. / Health Studies / M.A. (Health Studies)
|
38 |
Development of a taxonomy for visual literacy in the molecular life sciences.Mnguni, Lindelani Elphas. January 2007 (has links)
The use of external representations (ERs) such as diagrams and animations in science
education, particularly in the Molecular Life Sciences (MLS), has rapidly increased over
the past decades. Research shows that ERs have a superior advantage over text alone for
teaching and learning. Research has also indicated a number of concerns coupled with the
use of ERs for education purposes. Such problems emanate from the mode of
presentation and/or inability to use ERs. Regarding the later, a number of factors have
been identified as major causes of student difficulties and they include visual literacy as
one of the major factors. Given that little has been done to understand the nature of VL in
the MLS the current study was conducted with the general aim of investigating this area
and devising a way to measure the visual literacy levels of our students. More
specifically, this study addressed the following research questions: i) What is the nature
of visual literacy in MLS?; ii) Can specific levels of visual literacy be defined in the
MLS?; and iii) Is a taxonomy a useful way of representing the levels of visual literacy for
MLS? To respond to these questions, the current literature was used to define the nature
of visual literacy and the visualization skills (VSs). These were then used to develop a
Visual Literacy Test made up on probes in the context of Biochemistry. In these probes,
the VSs were incorporated. The test was administered to 3rd year Biochemistry students
who were also interviewed. Results were analysed qualitatively and quantitatively. The
later analysis utilized the Rasch model to generate an item difficulty map. The results of
the current study show that visual literacy is multifaceted in nature and is context based
in that it requires specific propositional knowledge. In line with this, it was found that
visual literacy is expressed through a cognitive process of visualization which requires
VSs. Based on the performance of these skills, learners’ optimal visual literacy in the
context of the MLS can be defined. Such performance can be assessed through the
development of probes in the Biochemistry context. Furthermore, the current research has
shown that using probes, the difficulty degree of each VS can be determined. In this
instance, the Rasch model is a preferred method of ranking VSs in the context of
Biochemistry in order of difficulty. From this, it was shown that given the uniqueness of
each skill’s degree of difficulty, each skill can thus be regarded as a level of visual
literacy. Such levels were defined in terms of the norm difficulty obtained in the current
study. Given the multifaceted nature of visual literacy, the current study adopted the view
that there are infinite number of VSs and hence the number of levels of visual literacy.
From the variation in the degree of difficulty, the study showed that there are nonvisualization
and visualization type difficulties which contribute to the differences in
visual literacy levels between Biochemistry students. In addition to this, the current study
showed that visual literacy in the MLS can be presented through a taxonomy. Such a
taxonomy can be used to determine the level of each VS, its name and definition, typical
difficulties found in the MLS as well as the visualization stage at which each skill is
performed. Furthermore, this taxonomy can be used to design models, assess students’
visual literacy, identify and inform the remediation of students’ visualization difficulties.
While the study has successfully defined the nature of visual literacy for the MLS and
presented visual literacy in a taxonomy, more work is required to further understand
visual literacy for the MLS, a field where visual literacy is very prevalent. / Thesis (M.Sc.) - University of KwaZulu-Natal, Pietermaritzburg, 2007.
|
39 |
The presentation and interpretation of arrow symbolism in biology diagrams at secondary-level.Du Plessis, Lynn. January 2006 (has links)
The literature contains conflicting ideas about the effectiveness of diagrams, and their constituent
symbolism as teaching and learning tools. In addition, only limited research has been specifically
conducted on the presentation and interpretation of arrow symbolism used in biology diagrams, let
alone on the nature, source and remediation of student difficulties caused by arrows. On the basis
of this limited research and 30 years of experience of teaching biology at secondary-level, the
author suspected that students might have difficulties interpreting arrow symbolism in diagrams
used as explanatory tools and decided to thoroughly investigate this issue. The hypothesis,
'Secondary-level students have difficulty with the use of arrow symbolism in biology diagrams' was
formulated and the following broad research questions defined to address the hypothesis:
1. How much of a problem is arrow symbolism in diagrams?
2. How effectively is arrow symbolism used in diagrams to promote the communication of intended
ideas?
3. To what extent does the design of arrow symbolism in diagrams influence students '
interpretation and difficulties?
4. How can the emerging empirical data and ideas from literature be combined to illustrate the
process of interpretation of arrow symbolism?
5. What measures can be suggested for improving the presentation and interpretation of arrow
symbolism in biology diagrams at secondary-level?
To address Research question 1, a content analysis of all arrow symbolism in seven popular
secondary-level biology textbooks was undertaken. This revealed a wide diversity of arrow styles,
spatial organisations, purposes and meanings that could be confusing to students. These results
suggested the need for an evaluation of the effectiveness of arrow symbolism (Research question
2). As there was no definitive set of guidelines available for specifically evaluating arrows, general
guidelines from the literature on diagrams were used to develop a set of 10 criteria, to evaluate the
syntactic, semantic and pragmatic dimensions of arrow symbolism, which were validated by
selected educators, students and a graphic design expert. Application of the criteria (which
constituted expert opinion) to the arrow symbolism used in 614 realistic, stylised and abstract
diagram types, revealed a relatively high incidence (30%) of inappropriately presented arrow
designs that could mislead students. To establish whether this problem could be the cause of
student difficulties, and to thereby address Research question 3, a stylised and an abstract diagram
were selected and evaluated according to the criteria. The results of the evaluation were compared
to the responses given by 174 students to a range of written and interview probes and student modified
diagrams. In this way, student performance was correlated with expert opinion. The
results confirmed that students experience a wide range of difficulties (26 categories) when
interpreting arrow symbolism, with some (12 categories) being attributable to inappropriately
presented arrow symbolism and others (14 categories) to student-related processing skills and
strategies at both surface- and deeper-levels of reasoning. To address question 4, the emerging
empirical data from the evaluation and student studies was combined with a wide range of
literature, to inform the development of a 3-level, non-tiered model of the process of interpretation
of arrow symbolism in diagrams. As this model emphasised the importance of both arrow
presentation in diagrams and arrow interpretation by students, it could be used as an effective
explanatory tool as well as a predictive tool to identify sources of difficulty with the use of arrow
symbolism. This model was, in turn, used to inform the compilation of a range of guidelines for
improving the presentation and interpretation of arrow symbolism, and so target Research question
5. These, and other guidelines grounded in the data and relevant literature, were suggested for all
role players, including students, educators, textbook writers, graphic artists and researchers, to use
as remedial tools. Future research should focus on the implementation of these guidelines and
studying their effectiveness for improving the presentation and interpretation of diagrams with
arrow and other types of symbolism. / Thesis (Ph.D.)-University of KwaZulu-Natal, Pietermaritzburg, 2006.
|
40 |
The Fixed v. Variable Sampling Interval Shewhart X-Bar Control Chart in the Presence of Positively Autocorrelated DataHarvey, Martha M. (Martha Mattern) 05 1900 (has links)
This study uses simulation to examine differences between fixed sampling interval (FSI) and variable sampling interval (VSI) Shewhart X-bar control charts for processes that produce positively autocorrelated data. The influence of sample size (1 and 5), autocorrelation parameter, shift in process mean, and length of time between samples is investigated by comparing average time (ATS) and average number of samples (ANSS) to produce an out of control signal for FSI and VSI Shewhart X-bar charts. These comparisons are conducted in two ways: control chart limits pre-set at ±3σ_x / √n and limits computed from the sampling process. Proper interpretation of the Shewhart X-bar chart requires the assumption that observations are statistically independent; however, process data are often autocorrelated over time. Results of this study indicate that increasing the time between samples decreases the effect of positive autocorrelation between samples. Thus, with sufficient time between samples the assumption of independence is essentially not violated. Samples of size 5 produce a faster signal than samples of size 1 with both the FSI and VSI Shewhart X-bar chart when positive autocorrelation is present. However, samples of size 5 require the same time when the data are independent, indicating that this effect is a result of autocorrelation. This research determined that the VSI Shewhart X-bar chart signals increasingly faster than the corresponding FSI chart as the shift in the process mean increases. If the process is likely to exhibit a large shift in the mean, then the VSI technique is recommended. But the faster signaling time of the VSI chart is undesirable when the process is operating on target. However, if the control limits are estimated from process samples, results show that when the process is in control the ARL for the FSI and the ANSS for the VSI are approximately the same, and exceed the expected value when the limits are fixed.
|
Page generated in 0.1409 seconds