Spelling suggestions: "subject:"cognitive dissonance"" "subject:"cognitive dissonances""
31 |
An axiomatic representation of Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance : an end and a beginning /Alexander, Sue Clark January 1971 (has links)
No description available.
|
32 |
Social and self perceptions of attitudes as a function of relevancy information /Wells, Gary Leroy January 1977 (has links)
No description available.
|
33 |
An experimental study of certain relationships between debaters' attitudes and win-loss recordsBarnes, Vernon Leray. January 1964 (has links)
Call number: LD2668 .T4 1964 B26 / Master of Science
|
34 |
UTILIZING THE BEHAVIOR-ATTITUDE RELATIONSHIP TO ENHANCE SELF-ESTEEM (SELF-PERCEPTION, SELF-REWARD, SELF-FOCUS, SELF-AWARENESS).Burling, John, 1956- January 1986 (has links)
No description available.
|
35 |
Reward magnitude and liking for instrumental activityLeventhal, Gerald S. (Gerald Seymour), 1936- 01 February 2017 (has links)
It is commonly assumed that the more an object facilitates attainment
of important goals, the more it is liked. Dissonance theory yields an opposite prediction. A two process conception of the relation between reward and attitude which reconciles these views is presented. A V-curve relation between reward magnitude and liking for activity instrumental to reward attainment is hypothesized. At the point where the relative number of dissonant cognitions is psychologically insignificant or zero, a direct satisfaction process supplants the dissonance process. The slope of the reward-attitude curve shifts from negative to positive at this transition point.
In an experiment designed to test this hypothesis students were offered 2, 4, or 3 experimental credit points for committing themselves to three hours of work at a highly repetitious task. Experimental credit points contribute to students' final course grades and are therefore highly valued by subjects. The instructions were varied such that both the task and the subject’s part in the experiment were placed in either a neutral or a negative light (Neutral or Negative Instructions conditions). Regardless of the Instructions used, 8 Credit groups expressed greater liking for the activity and more pleasure with participating than 4 Credit groups. Contrary to expectation. Affect scores for 2 Credit Neutral subjects were similar to those of 4 Credit Neutral subjects and significantly lower than those of 8 Credit Neutral subjects. Several possible explanations of the lack of difference between 2 and 4 Credit Neutral groups are discussed. It is suggested that the reward-attitude function is U-shaped rather than V-shaped and that these two groups both He in a relatively flat region of the curve in which dissonance and direct satisfaction effects operate simultaneously to increase attitudinal positivity. Though Negative Instructions (administered to one 4 Credit and
one 8 Credit group) were expected to produce an inverse relation between reward and liking, they simply resulted in a uniform drop in Affect Scale
scores. The Instructions variable had no impact on Desired Frequency of Rest Period, a less direct index of attitude. For the reward manipulation an opposite trend occurred. Varying the number of credit points had a relatively stronger effect on Desired Frequency of Rest scores and a relatively weaker (though still significant) effect on the Affect Scales. This result suggests that the Instructions variable actually failed to alter activity attractiveness. The effects of Negative Instructions on Affect Scale ratings could have been produced either by subjects* tendency to conform to the experimenter's apparent expectations or by the decrease of inter-personal situational restraints against public expression of dislike for the experiment in the experimenter's presence.
Neither acceptance nor rejection of the general two-process hypothesis of attitude formation is warranted by these data. Nevertheless, the experiment clearly demonstrates that reward magnitude and attitudes toward activities instrumental in securing the regard are positively related under certain conditions, a finding which suggests an important restriction on the generality of Festinger’s dissonance theory. / This thesis was digitized as part of a project begun in 2014 to increase the number of Duke psychology theses available online. The digitization project was spearheaded by Ciara Healy.
|
36 |
Remembering and forgetting concurrently new benefits of high working memory span /AuBuchon, Angela M. Cowan, Nelson. January 2009 (has links)
Title from PDF of title page (University of Missouri--Columbia, viewed on Feb 17, 2010). The entire thesis text is included in the research.pdf file; the official abstract appears in the short.pdf file; a non-technical public abstract appears in the public.pdf file. Thesis advisor: Dr. Nelson Cowan. Includes bibliographical references.
|
37 |
The use of dissonance theory to modify patient teaching behaviorCastellan, Deborah Mary, 1949- January 1977 (has links)
No description available.
|
38 |
The escalation of aggression in people as measured by the progression of insult severityMotoi, Gabriela January 2009 (has links)
Research investigating the underlying causes and factors involved in violence and aggression has suggested there is a tendency for aggression to escalate as a means to justify prior aggression. In addition, past research has also examined the effect of perceived similarity towards the target of aggression on intensity and escalation of aggression. This study looked at the relationship between initial level of aggression and the escalation of aggression and at perceived similarity to the target of aggression as a possible factor influencing this escalation. Individuals engaging in severe initial aggression who experience higher perceived similarity to their targets of aggression should be more prone to justifying their actions and so might escalate more. To examine this, subjects could administer any of 10 levels of negative reinforcement (insults) to a learner for incorrect responses. Half of the subjects were required to practice this procedure with a mild and half with a severe insult. Results indicated that an effect of perceived similarity emerged, with individuals using less severe insults when perceived similarity to the learner was high. Contrary to predictions, high-perceived similarity to the learner stemmed escalation for participants insulting the learner with a severe insult initially. Moreover, participants who insulted with a mild insult initially escalated in their aggression when perceived similarity was high. In addition, an interaction effect of gender and perceived similarity was found, with men engaging in more severe subsequent aggression than women when perceived similarity to the target of aggression is high. The limitations, further directions, and implications of this study are discussed.
|
39 |
Functional magnetic resonance imaging of rationalization understanding neural and psychological mechanisms of cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making /Jarcho, Johanna Molly, January 1900 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--UCLA, 2008. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references.
|
40 |
The role of volition in attitude change resulting from hearing an attacking speechFett, John H. January 1967 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Wisconsin, 1967. / Typescript. Vita. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references.
|
Page generated in 0.0747 seconds