Spelling suggestions: "subject:"cognitive learning theory."" "subject:"aognitive learning theory.""
11 |
Evaluation of a social cognitive theory-based adolescent physical activity intervention Plan for exercise, plan for health /Stevens, Emily Claire, January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Ohio State University, 2006. / Title from first page of PDF file. Includes bibliographical references (p. 272-279).
|
12 |
The many faces of neurocognitive development behavior and neurocorrelates of holistic face processing /Paparello, Silvia. January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of California, San Diego, 2007. / Title from first page of PDF file (viewed January 14, 2008). Available via ProQuest Digital Dissertations. Vita. Includes bibliographical references. Faces are central stimuli in our everyday life, hence, face processing is a sophisticated and highly specialized cognitive ability, at which adults are experts and children are proficient. Unlike other visuospatial abilities, face perception develops very slowly, becoming adult-like only well into adolescence. Some performance disparities between children and adults may reflect differences in general cognitive abilities, such as attention and memory. Alternatively, performance differences can be attributed to specific cognitive strategies implemented during face processing by different age groups; or to the interaction between the improvement of general abilities throughout development and the refinement of face specific cognitive strategies. The intent of the current studies was to further assess the development of and relationship between cognitive strategies in face processing. Specifically, we investigated the behavior and neurocorrelates associated with holistic face processing in children (8- to 11-year-olds), adolescents, and adults, utilizing the composite face effect. The task requires participants to engage in both holistic and featural processing, but certain trials (aligned-same) elicit a visual illusion called the composite face effect (CFE, calculated as difference between misaligned-same and aligned-same trials), which is considered an index of holistic processing. All age groups (adults, adolescents, 8- to 9-year-olds, 10- to 11-year-olds) showed a CFE, suggesting reliance on holistic processing. Notably, about half of the 8- to 11-year-old children displayed adult-like behavior and adult-like CFE, suggesting their reliance on holistic processing. However, the other half of the children performed below-chance on aligned-same trials, displayed an extremely large CFE, and a significant difference between different trials, suggesting reliance on a featural strategy. Thus child age groups were regrouped according to their accuracy performance on the hardest condition (aligned-same trials) into high performing and low performing children. We hypothesize that the aligned-same trials were too taxing for low-performing children, thus they fell back into relying on simpler strategies such as a difference-detection featural strategy. In order to further investigate the CFE behavioral differences between age and performance groups, we completed an imaging study. For the fMRI study children were grouped by performance rather than age following the results of our behavioral study. Overall, our imaging results for the CFE, thus for holistic processing, resembled behavioral results in that adult and high performing child groups revealed a similar (but not identical) whole-brain pattern of activation, whereas the low performing child group showed a distinctive pattern of activation for the composite face effect. Adults and high performing children showed a pattern of activation spanning frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes. In contrast, low performing children revealed a pattern of activation that spanned frontal, temporal, cingulate, and cerebellar regions. Brain areas typically associated with face processing, such as the right fusiform gyrus and right inferior temporal gyrus did not reach significance for the low performing child group. These differences may be attributable to the use of different cognitive strategies. However, the extent of frontal and cingulate cortex activation in low performing children may also suggest that because the task was especially difficult for them, working memory resources were particularly taxed, thus affecting the neural network engaged. Importantly, not only were performance differences associated with distinct neurocorrelates (i.e., differing profiles for low performing children vs. high performing children and adults), but age differences also had an appreciable effect. In fact, high performing children did not significantly differ from adults in the behavioral CFE, but did show differences in the neural CFE.
|
13 |
A kaleidoscope of decisions using cognitive flexibility theory to advance a novice ESOL teacher's scaffolding expertise /Taylor, Donna Lester. January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Georgia State University, 2007. / Joyce E. Many, committee chair; Mona W. Matthews, Dana L. Fox, Lori N. Elliott, committee members. Electronic text (190 p. : ill. (some col.)) : digital, PDF file. Description based on contents viewed June 19, 2008; title from file title page. Includes bibliographical references (p. 177-187).
|
14 |
Modeling higher order thinking in teacher preparation : an examination of the relationships between course objectives, classroom discoure, and assessment /Ball, Anna L. January 2002 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Missouri-Columbia, 2002. / Typescript. Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 130-138). Also available on the Internet.
|
15 |
Modeling higher order thinking in teacher preparation an examination of the relationships between course objectives, classroom discoure, and assessment /Ball, Anna L. January 2002 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Missouri-Columbia, 2002. / Typescript. Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 130-138). Also available on the Internet.
|
16 |
A social cognitivist view of hypermedia learningCortese, Juliann. January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Ohio State University, 2005. / Title from first page of PDF file. Document formatted into pages; contains xv, 201 p.; also includes graphics. Includes bibliographical references (p. 191-201). Available online via OhioLINK's ETD Center
|
17 |
Complex instruction giving students the education they deserve /Tobias, Cindel K. January 2010 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (M.I.T.)--The Evergreen State College, 2010. / Title from title screen (viewed 7/7/2010). Includes bibliographical references (leaves 161-167).
|
18 |
Mother's social cognitions and discipline responses differences between physical and relational aggression /Senich, Samantha, January 2006 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (M.A. in human development)--Washington State University, May 2006. / Includes bibliographical references (p. 41-50).
|
19 |
The interaction of goal and temporal shifts in situation modelsCopeland, Angelina M. January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Notre Dame, 2006. / Thesis directed by Gabriel A. Radvansky for the Department of Psychology. "July 2006." Includes bibliographical references (leaves 82-86).
|
20 |
Improving mathematics instruction and teachers' decision making a case study in Thailand /Foochai Komalabutr. McCarthy, John R., Heyl, Barbara Sherman, January 1995 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Illinois State University, 1995. / Title from title page screen, viewed April 24, 2006. Dissertation Committee: John R. McCarthy, Barbara S. Heyl (co-chairs), Patricia H. Klass, Cherly A. Lubinski. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 107-114) and abstract. Also available in print.
|
Page generated in 0.0871 seconds