Spelling suggestions: "subject:"conditioned response."" "subject:"oonditioned response.""
41 |
Initial performance expectations, strength of those expectations, and social interaction behavior of subordinates : their effect on the development process in leader-member exchange theoryStilwell, C. Dean 12 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
42 |
The classical conditioning of positive and negative attitude change /Brender, William January 1971 (has links)
No description available.
|
43 |
The anticipatory modification of the conditioning of a fear response in humans.Surwit, Richard S. January 1972 (has links)
No description available.
|
44 |
Amount of escape vs. no-escape training : a variable in conditioning "learned helplessness"Ryman, Fred L. January 1973 (has links)
Four groups of rats (10 rats per group), composing an independent subjects design, were exposed to various amounts of escape training (escape contingent upon a fixed-ratio of 3 lever presses) and inescapable shook, prior to being tested on an escape task identical to the one on which they were trained. Interest was focused on the length of time required for, the number of, and the consistency of successful escapes as measures of disruption in responding. The High conditions consisted of 100 escape training trials and 200 seconds of inescapable shock while the Low conditions consisted of 5 escape training trials and 20 seconds of inescapable shock. The four possible combinations of the escape training and inescapable shock composed the four groups. A summary of the results indicated the production of learned helplessness (as measured by the amount of disruption) was not significantly effected by either the amount of escape pretraining or the amount of inescapable shook given the subjects.
|
45 |
The efficacy of N-stage testing versus intermediate testing in the formation of equivalence classes of chemical elementsVieitez, Doreen E. January 1994 (has links)
A set of stimuli comprises an equivalence class if the three relations of reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity are present (Sidman & Tailby, 1982). Although behaviorological researchers have suggested that the training and testing sequence may affect equivalence class formation, this has not been studied directly. The purpose of this study was to investigate the efficacy of two different arrangements of training and testing on the formation of classes of equivalent stimuli. Five middle school students were taught eight conditional discriminations with four classes of stimuli, three stimuli per class (A1B1C1, A2B2C2, A3B3C3, and A4B4C4). A fifth class of stimuli, A5B5C5, was used as control stimuli. Experimental stimuli consisted of five chemical elements, with three attributes per element (name, symbol, and atomic number). The formation of three-member equivalence classes was evaluated by testing students for symmetrical and transitive conditional discriminations involved in the training relations. Two phase arrangements were used with each student. Phase Arrangement I (incorporating n-stage testing) was as follows: (a) AM; (b) B1C1; (c) A1B1 and B1C1 mixed together; (d) test (A1C1, C1A1, A5C5, and C5A5); (e) AM; (f) B2C2; (g) A2B2 and B2C2 mixed together; (h) test (A2C2, C2A2, A5C5, and C5A5); (i) AM, B1C1, AM, and B2C2 all mixed together; and (j) test (AlCl, C1A1, A2C2, C2A2, A5C5, and C5A5). Phase Arrangement II was similar, except that test phases (d) and (h) were eliminated. Stimulus classes A3B3C3 and A4B4C4 were arranged analogously to provide a counterbalanced design. One student performed equally well on equivalence tests with both phase arrangements. Two students performed slightly better on equivalence tests with intermediate testing. One student's performance on equivalence tests demonstrated no equivalence class formation with n-stage testing and much more accurate, although varied, responding with intermediate testing. A 5th student who did not meet the pretest requirements for the study was nevertheless allowed to complete the experimental tasks because his test results were unusual. His first test score was far below chance level, but scores improved with subsequent testing. The results suggest that an intermediate testing arrangement may decrease intersubject variability and, for some individuals, may improve equivalence test performance. / Department of Special Education
|
46 |
Interest inventory items as attitude eliciting stimuli in classical conditioning: a test of the A-R-D theoryGross, Michael Carlon January 1970 (has links)
Typescript. / Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Hawaii, 1970. / Bibliography: leaves [50]-53. / v, 53 l tables
|
47 |
The effect of conditioned stimulus intensity in classical conditioning of the common carpWoodard, William Theodore January 1966 (has links)
Typescript. / Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Hawaii, 1966. / Bibliography: leaves [103]-107. / viii, 107 l illus., tables
|
48 |
A comparison of procedures for unpairing conditioned reflexive establishing operationsKettering, Tracy Lynne, January 2008 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Ohio State University, 2008. / Title from first page of PDF file. Includes bibliographical references (p. 119-124).
|
49 |
Differential reinforcing effects of onset and offset of stimulation on the operant behavior of normals, neurotics and psychopathsWiesen, Allen E. January 1965 (has links)
Thesis--University of Florida, 1965. / eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Bibliography: leaves 45-46.
|
50 |
The effect of noncontingent pretraining on the development of an instrumental discrimination between differential magnitudes of reinforcementFlaherty, Charles Foster, January 1967 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison, 1967. / eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references.
|
Page generated in 0.0792 seconds