Spelling suggestions: "subject:"group decision"" "subject:"croup decision""
211 |
Can lay leaders at OakWood Church successfully identify, make informed decisions about solving, and accept ownership for the solutions to key missional problems in the area of outreach?Sheppard, David R. January 1999 (has links)
Project Thesis (D. Min.)--Denver Conservative Baptist Seminary, 1999. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 273-284).
|
212 |
Casting a wide net decision-making in a Mexican marine park /Peterson, Nicole Dyan. January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of California, San Diego, 2005. / Title from first page of PDF file (viewed January 11, 2006). Available via ProQuest Digital Dissertations. Vita. Includes bibliographical references ( p. 303-315).
|
213 |
General education teachers' perceptions of their involvement in the IEP processSummers, Kathryn Lynn. Smith, Paula J. January 1995 (has links)
Thesis (Ed. D.)--Illinois State University, 1995. / Title from title page screen, viewed May 5, 2006. Dissertation Committee: Paula J. Smith (chair), Mack L. Bowen, Norma C. DeMario, Ronald S. Halinski, Mark E. Swerdlik. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 72-77) and abstract. Also available in print.
|
214 |
Conflict and creativity in student writing groups a case study investigation /Lamonica, Claire Coleman. Neuleib, Janice. January 1996 (has links)
Thesis (D.A.)--Illinois State University, 1996. / Title from title page screen, viewed May 23, 2006. Dissertation Committee: Janice Grace Neuleib (chair), James Robert Kalmbach, Heather Ann Brodie Graves, John Francis Cragen. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 215-222) and abstract. Also available in print.
|
215 |
'n Ondersoek na die rol van strukturele groepfaktore en groepprosesse met betrekking tot bestuurspaninnoverendheidBotha, S. (Sonja) 03 1900 (has links)
Thesis (MA)--University of Stellenbosch, 2002. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: Innovation and creativity are established research areas but both are relatively new
research areas within the field of organisational psychology. Early research on
innovation and creativity were primarily the focus area of academics in related social
sciences disciplines. Research on innovation and creativity was integrated with the
orientations and perspectives of organisational psychologists over a period of time. In
other words the development and identification of innovation as a success factor of the
work environment is something that took place over decades. Changes in the competitive
environment over the last few decades contributed to the increasing interest in the
concept of innovation. Today managers recognise innovation as a necessity that needs to
be managed and developed.
Three levels of analyses (the individual, group and organisation) can be identified in the
innovation literature. Until recently researchers focused primarily on the individual and
the organisation as units of analysis. As a results of this, research that focuses on the
individual and the organisation are quite comprehensive compared to studies that focus
on the group as a unit of analysis. The utilisation of teams have increased in
organisations, but our knowledge of the factors and dimensions that determine group
innovation are still restricted. The complex nature of group innovation could perhaps be a contributing factor in terms of the amount of research studies that have been done on
group innovation.
It is especially in this regard that this study sets out to thoroughly investigate existing
group innovation literature and to combine this with an investigation into the possible
contributions of related social psychological research (for example group decision
making, intragroup conflict and creative problem solving) to the innovation literature.
Each one of the three units of analysis (individual, group and organisation) has a unique
impact on group innovation, but not one of them explains exclusively the innovativeness
of a team. An interdependent relationship exists between these factors. This stresses the
complexity of the concept and as a result of this, researchers are encouraged to
investigate the possible contributions of related social psychological themes to the
innovation research and literature.
In this regard this study focused extensively on a input, process and output model of
group innovation by West and Anderson (1996) to determine to what extend some of the
aspects of the model explained the innovativeness of a group. This is a comprehensive
model and for the purposes of this study only certain aspects of the model were included
in the research process. The size of the team and team tenure were the two input
elements that were included in the study. The process elements of the model includes
clarity and commitment to team objectives, participation, task orientation and support for
innovation. In terms of the output aspects of the model the focus was on the following:
how radical, effective, new and extensive the innovation is, as well as the number of
innovations per team. The results of the study indicate that there are a relationship
between the variables of West and Anderson's input, process and output model (1996)
and the innovativeness of the team. The results of this study indicate that the model by
West and Anderson is a valuable contribution to the research literature on group
innovation behaviour. Although group innovation has not been researched extensively, it seems that group
innovation is starting to make an impact on the organisational psychology domain and
that it will be a central aspect of the research agenda of organisational psychologists in
future. If we are able to establish a sound theoretical basis organisations will be able to
understand and predict group innovation behaviour.
It can be concluded that this study made a contribution to the research literature on group
innovation behaviour in terms of an awareness of the impact and necessity of innovation
in today's work environment, and also in extending the existing knowledge base of group
innovation behaviour / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Alhoewel innovasie al VIr 'n geruime tyd as navorsingsdomein gevestig IS, het
organisasiesielkundiges eers redelik onlangs aan innovasie en kreatiwiteit begin aandag
skenk. Een van die redes waarom dit so lank geneem het voordat innovasie as 'n
waardige wetenskaplike navorsingsonderwerp besef was, was die feit dat vroeëre
navorsing oor hierdie onderwerp deur akademici in ander sosiaal wetenskaplike
dissiplines uitgevoer was. Die integrering van sodanige navorsing met
organisasiesielkundiges se perspektiewe en oriëntasies het skynbaar slegs met verloop
van tyd plaasgevind.
Die ontwikkeling van innovasie as 'n verskynsel van die werksomgewing het dus
stelselmatig oor dekades heen plaasgevind. Die impak van omgewingsveranderinge op
organisasies het ook oor die afgelope paar dekades beduidende belangstelling ten opsigte
van innovasie ontsluit. Innovasie, as 'n komponent van organisatoriese-kompeterendheid
en -effektiwiteit, is vandag 'n noodsaaklikheid wat toenemend deur hedendaagse
bedryfsleiers ondersteun en bestuur word.
Binne die werksomgewing kom innovasie op drie verskillende vlakke (die individu, die
groep en die organisasie) voor. Tot dusver het innovasienavorsers egter in hoofsaak op
die individu en organisasie as ontledingseenheid gefokus. Navorsing wat die individu of
organisasie as ontledingseenheid gebruik, is volop in vergelyking met studies wat die
groep as ontledingseenheid gebruik. Met ander woorde, alhoewel die gebruikmaking van
spanne in organisasies toeneem, is ons kennis van die faktore en dimensies wat spaninnoverendheid bepaal, steeds beperk. Die beperkte teoretiese basis van bestaande
groepinnovasienavorsing kan deels aan die omvangryke en komplekse aard van die
verskynsel toegeskryf word.
Dit is juis in hierdie opsig dat hierdie studie dit ten doel gestel het om bestaande
groepinnovasieliteratuur deeglik te ondersoek en om vas te stel tot watter mate verwante
sosiaal-sielkundige navorsing (onder andere groepbesluitneming, intragroepkonflik en
kreatiewe probleemoplossing) 'n bydrae tot die innovasieliteratuur kan lewer.
Elk van die drie ontledingsvlakke (individu, groep, organisasie) het 'n unieke effek op die
innoverendheid van 'n span, maar nie een van hierdie kategorieë verklaar uitsluitlik die
innoverendheid van 'n span nie. Die interafhanklike verhouding van hierdie faktore
beklemtoon die kompleksiteit van die verskynsel en dit moedig toekomstige navorsers
aan om die nuttige bydraes van verwante sosiaal-sielkundige temas te ondersoek.
In hierdie verband is daar ook breedvoerig gebruik gemaak van 'n inset, proses en uitset
model van groepinnoverendheid deur West en Anderson (1996), ten einde vas te stel tot
watter mate sekere aspekte van die model die innoverendheid van 'n span voorspel.
Vanweë die omvangrykheid van die model is slegs sekere veranderlikes in die navorsing
ingesluit. In terme van die insette is daar op grootte en spandienstydperk gefokus. Die
proses faset van die model verwys na die duidelikheid en verbondenheid tot doelwitte,
deelname, taakoriëntasie en ondersteuning vir innovasie. Ten opsigte van die uitset faset
van die model is daar op die volgende aspekte gefokus: die radikaalheid,
omvangrykheid, nuutheid en effektiwiteit van die innovasie asook die getal innovasie
items binne die groep. Die resultate van die studie dui daarop dat daar wel 'n verband
tussen hierdie veranderlikes van West en Anderson (1996) se model en die
innoverendheid van groepe bestaan. Hierdie model van groepinnoverendheid blyk vanuit
die resultate van hierdie studie, en vorige studies, nuttig te wees vir die bestudering van
groepinnovasiegedrag. Alhoewel daar tot dusver min lig gewerp is op die faktore wat groepinnoverendheid
bepaal, wil dit voorkom asof groepinnovasienavorsing 'n sentrale faset van die
organisasiesielkunde se navorsingsagenda in die toekoms sal wees. Die vestiging en
uitbreiding van 'n teoretiese basis salorganisasies in staat stelom die innovasiegedrag
van spanne beter te verstaan en te voorspel.
Ten slotte kan dit vermeld word dat hierdie studie In bydrae gelewer het tot
groepinnovasiegedrag, nie net in terme van 'n bewusmaking van die impak en
noodsaaklikheid van innovasie in die hedendaagse werksomgewing nie, maar ook in
terme van die uitbreiding van die bestaande kennisbasis oor groepinnovasiegedrag.
|
216 |
Análise da influência do compartilhamento da informação e da complexidade da tarefa na decisão em grupo / Analysis on the influence of information sharing and task complexity in group decision makingVisentini, Monize Sâmara 18 January 2010 (has links)
Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior / Decision making is crucial for enterprises, being frequently done by groups. However, the way information is shared among groups, as well as the complexity of the task being analyzed might affect the quality of group decision. Thus, the following research aims to identify how
information sharing and task complexity influence on group decision. Besides, it is aimed to (a) validate a decisive task related to information sharing and task complexity; b) verify the way information sharing affects taking decisions in groups and; c) illustrate the effect of task
complexity in group decisive process. Four hypotheses were drawn in order to corroborate these objectives. The methodology applied was an almost-experimental research conducted in a laboratory, having as independent variables the information sharing and task complexity; and as a dependent variable, group decision. In order to contemplate those variables, the applied task has been adapted from Stasser and Stewart (1992) and Fraidin (2004), and is denominated Murder Mystery . This task went through a process of reverse translation,
cultural adaptation, and two validations corroborating the first specific objective of this research. It was used a system for data collection developed specifically for the experiment, STAR Individual x SER Grupo . Besides, group discussions were followed by observers previously trained and recorded for further analysis. Qualitative and quantitative analyses were conducted. The number of undergraduate students taking part in the experiment was
144. Concerning specific objectives, it was observed that there were no differences between the quality of the decision from both groups, independently of the type of information sharing, good decisions were taken. Fisher s test has been applied to the decisions with total and
partial sharing. It was also compared the decisions taken by groups that mad a simple task and the ones of a complex one, with no statistically significance for Fisher s test, leading to the fact that there is no difference between the quality of the decision taken by groups that made a
simple task and groups that made complex ones. However, the more a task is complex; the less information is discussed by a member of the group, jeopardizing decision making. All the research objectives were reached. In a general sense, through testing four hypotheses, there
was no influence of information sharing and task complexity in group decision, opposing a great part of the literature, indicating that more studies on this topic are necessary. / O ato de decidir é crucial para as empresas, sendo freqüente que grupos o realizem. Entretanto, a forma como a informação é compartilhada entre os membros do grupo, bem como o grau de complexidade da tarefa que está sendo analisada podem afetar a qualidade da decisão grupal. Dessa forma, esta pesquisa visa identificar como o compartilhamento da
informação e a complexidade da tarefa influenciam a decisão em grupo. Além disso, ainda busca-se (a) validar uma tarefa decisória relacionada ao compartilhamento de informações e a
complexidade da tarefa; b) verificar como o compartilhamento de informações afeta a tomada de decisão em grupo e; c) ilustrar o efeito da complexidade da tarefa no processo decisório grupal. Quatro hipóteses foram traçadas a fim de corroborar esses objetivos. Como metodologia de investigação realizou-se um quase-experimento em laboratório, tendo como
variáveis independentes o compartilhamento da informação e a complexidade da tarefa, e como variável dependente a decisão em grupo. Para contemplar essas variáveis, a tarefa
aplicada foi adaptada de Stasser e Stewart (1992) e Fraidin (2004), e denomina-se Mistério do Assassinato . Essa tarefa passou por um processo de tradução reversa, adaptação cultural e duas validações corroborando ao primeiro objetivo específico deste trabalho. Para a aplicação da mesma foi utilizado um sistema de coleta de dados desenvolvido especificamente para este experimento, o STAR Individual x SER Grupo. Além disso, as discussões dos grupos foram
acompanhadas por observadores previamente treinados e gravadas para posterior análise. Foram realizadas análises quantitativa e qualitativa dos dados. Ao total participaram do
experimento 144 estudantes de graduação. Com relação do aos objetivos específicos, observou-se, através do Teste de Fisher aplicado às decisões tomadas pelos grupos com
compartilhamento total e parcial, que não houve diferenças entre a qualidade da decisão de ambos os grupos, sendo que independentemente do tipo de compartilhamento da informação boas decisões foram tomadas. Também se comparou as decisões tomadas pelos grupos que
realizaram a tarefa simples e os que realizaram a tarefa complexa, não encontrando significância estatística para o Teste de Fisher, indicando não haver diferença entre a
qualidade da decisão tomada pelos grupos que realizaram tarefas simples e os grupos que realizaram tarefas complexas. Entretanto, verificou-se que quanto maior a complexidade da
tarefa menor número de informações discutidas pelo membro do grupo, o que pode prejudicar a tomada de decisão. Todos os objetivos da pesquisa foram atingidos. De um modo geral, a
partir do teste de quatro hipóteses, não se verificou influência do compartilhamento da informação e da complexidade da tarefa na decisão em grupo, contrariando grande parte da
literatura, o que indica que mais estudos acerca dessa temática tornam-se necessários.
|
217 |
Activités collaboratives et génération de comportements d'agents : moteur décisionnel s'appuyant sur un modèle de confiance / Collective activities and agent behavior : trust-based decision-making systemCallebert, Lucile 18 October 2016 (has links)
Lorsqu’ils travaillent en équipe, les humains ont rarement des comportements optimaux : ils peuvent faire des erreurs, manquer de motivation ou de compétence. Dans les domaines des environnements virtuels ou des systèmes multi-agents, de nombreux travaux ont cherché à reproduire les comportements d’équipes humaines : un agent représente alors un membre de l’équipe. Cependant, ces travaux ont très souvent pour objectif la performance de l’équipe, et non la fidélité des comportements produits. Pour former un apprenant en environnement virtuel à prêter attention et à s’adapter aux autres, nous avons cherché dans cette thèse à reproduire des comportements humains réalistes et non-optimaux de travail d’équipe. Plus particulièrement, nous nous sommes intéressés aux équipes auto-organisées, c’est-à-dire aux équipes dans lesquelles le pouvoir de décision est réparti entre les membres, et dans lesquelles l’organisation est implicite. Dans de telles équipes, l’organisation se fait non pas au travers des communications mais par l’observation et l’anticipation des comportements des autres. Pour s’organiser, chaque agent doit se demander ce qu’il est préférable de faire en fonction de ce que pourraient faire les autres, et donc se poser des questions telles que Ai-je confiance en la compétence de mon coéquipier pour faire cette tâche ? Les relations de confiance permettent donc à chacun de prendre en compte les autres. Pour générer de tels comportements, nous proposons un système permettant aux agents de raisonner d’une part sur un modèle de l’activité à effectuer et d’autre part sur les relations de confiance qui les lient aux autres agents de l’environnement. Dans ce cadre, notre première contribution porte sur l’augmentation du langage de description de l’activité Activity-DL de manière à permettre la description d’activités collectives. Nous proposons également des mécanismes de propagation de contraintes et d’informations qui faciliteront le raisonnement des agents. Ces contraintes et informations permettront par exemple aux agents de répondre à la question Avons-nous les compétences nécessaires pour faire la tâche qui permettra de réaliser notre but collectif ? Notre seconde contribution porte sur la proposition d’un modèle d’agent opérationnalisant le modèle de confiance de Mayer et al. (1995), sélectionné après une étude de la littérature en sciences humaines et sociales sur la confiance. La confiance d’un agent en un autre est décrite selon trois dimensions : l’intégrité, la bienveillance et les compétences. Chaque agent est donc défini par ces trois dimensions et a des croyances sur l’intégrité, la bienveillance et la compétence des autres agents. De plus chaque agent possède des buts qui lui sont personnels ainsi que des buts collectifs et devra donc choisir quel but privilégier. Finalement nous proposons un moteur décisionnel qui permet à chaque agent de calculer l’importance qu’il accorde à ses buts afin de sélectionner une tâche. Nous avons défini les mécanismes de calcul de l’importance des buts de manière à modéliser l’influence sur l’agent de ses croyances sur les autres, et pour sélectionner une tâche, l’agent raisonne à la fois sur les modèles d’activité et sur ses attentes à propos du comportement des autres, également générées à partir des croyances de l’agent sur les autres. Nous avons implémenté notre système et constaté qu’il répond à nos objectifs de génération de comportements d’équipe réalistes et non optimaux. Nous avons également conduit une évaluation perceptive préliminaire au cours de laquelle les participants ont notamment été capables de percevoir la confiance ou le manque de confiance d’un agent en un autre grâce à son comportement. / When working in teams, humans rarely display optimal behaviors: they sometimes make mistakes, lack motivation or competence. In virtual environments or in multi-agent systems, many studies have tried to reproduce human teamwork: each agent acts as a team member. However, the main objective in those studies is the performance of the team: each agent should display optimal behavior, and the realism of those simulated behaviors is not a concern. To train someone in a virtual environment to pay attention to and to adapt to their teammates, we built a decision-making system for agents to display realistic and non-optimal behaviors. More specifically, we are interested in self-organized teams (i.e. teams where the decision power is decentralized among its members) and in implicit organization (i.e. when team members do not interact through communications but rather through the observation of others’ behaviors). In such a team, each agent has to think about what it should do given what others could do. Agents then have to ask themselves questions such as Do I trust my teammate’s competence to perform this task? Trust relationships therefore allow agents to take others into account. We propose a system that allows agents to reason, on the first hand, on models of the activity they have to do, and on the other hand, on trust relationships they share with others. In that context, we first augmented the Activity-Description Language so that it supports the description of collective activities. We also defined mechanisms for constraint generation that facilitates agent reasoning, by giving them the answer to questions like Do we have the required abilities to perform the task which will achieve our goal? We then proposed an agent model based on the model of interpersonal trust of Mayer et al. (1995) that we selected after a study of trust in social science. This model describes trust relationship with three dimensions: the trustor trusts the trustee’s integrity, benevolence and abilities. An agent is therefore defined through those three dimensions, and has a mental model of each other agent; i.e. has trust beliefs about others’ integrity, benevolence and abilities. Moreover each agent has both personal and collective goals (i.e. goals that are shared with other members of the team), and thus will have to decide which goal to focus on. Finally we proposed a decision-making system that allows an agent to compute the importance it gives to its goals and then to select a task. When computing goal importance, the agent is influenced by its trust beliefs about others, and to select a task, it reasons on the activity models and on its expectations about what others could do. Those expectations are generated from the agents’ trust beliefs. We implemented our system and observed that it produces realistic and non-optimal behaviors. We also conducted a preliminary perceptive evaluation which showed that participants were able to recognize one agent’s trust or lack of trust in another through the behaviors of the first one.
|
218 |
Eliciting the language of decision making through collaborative revision of compositionsPoliakoff, Nan Karen January 1990 (has links)
Written and oral second language pedagogy have undergone a major shift from the traditional focus on form and end-product to a focus on the process of creating meaning. However, recent research indicates that a reliance on process alone is insufficient, and that specific linguistic and knowledge-related demands must be made within a process if second language learners are to develop proficiency in oral and written expository discourse. Interactive decision-making activities in the classroom may present such demands, but the pre-constructed, somewhat artificial tasks typically included in adult ESL classes are generally directed toward the limited goal of stimulating more talk, without verifying their value for generating the language that is cognitively linked to decision-making. While collaborative revision of compositions has been used to aid decision-making during writing, its potential as a language learning activity has not previously been explored.
The current study examines collaborative revision of compositions as a tool for eliciting oral decision-making language in the ESL classroom. Eight adult ESL students participated in two collaborative composition-related tasks and two interactive "constructed decision" activities. Using repeated measures ANOVA, the composition related activities were found to be at least as effective as the constructed decision activities for eliciting decision-making language as measured by five categories of decision-making moves. Qualitative examination of the discourse indicated that the composition activities also offer a context for conscious reinforcement of principles of grammar and rhetoric.
The present results indicate that this activity may offer a natural alternative to pre-constructed decision-making tasks, and in doing so provides a context for the integration of oral and written language learning. In addition, by evaluating the discourse of activities in language terms, this research may help provide a bridge between processes and products not only for writing but for communicative language teaching. / Education, Faculty of / Graduate
|
219 |
Virtual design office: A collaborative unified modeling language toolTotapally, Hara 01 January 2001 (has links)
Real-time conferencing and collaborative computing is a great way to make developers more effective. This project is a collaborative framework development comprising configurable client and server components.
|
220 |
Face-to-face versus computer-mediated communication: An investigation of multiple outcomes across task-typesVillado, Anton James 01 January 2001 (has links)
A novel approach was used to investigate differences between groups using computer-mediated and face-to-face communication. In a laboratory setting, three-person groups completed three survival themed tasks. The tasks were related in that the output of a preliminary task became the input of the sequent task.
|
Page generated in 0.0814 seconds