• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 17
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • Tagged with
  • 22
  • 22
  • 8
  • 8
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Developmental aspects of the habituation of the skin conductance response to audtory stimuli

Guminski, Margaret Mary. January 1978 (has links)
Thesis--University of Wisconsin--Madison. / Typescript. Vita. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 71-77).
2

Factors affecting long-term habituation in Caenorhabditis elegans

Beck, Christine Daily O’Brien 11 1900 (has links)
The objective of these experiments was to explore long-term memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. This examination of memory in a simple organism with accessible genetics and a well understood biology may permit later work to define the cellular processes that underlie long-term memory. Habituation training with a vibrational stimulus was administered on Day 1, and the retention test of a block of stimuli was given 24 h after the end of training on Day 2. Long-term retention of habituation was evident as a lower level of responding on Day 2 relative to the level of responding on Day 2 of untrained controls or the initial level of responding of worms on Day 1. In Experiments 1 and 2, a habituation training protocol that produced long-term retention of habituation was established, and the effects of stimulus number, interstimulus interval (ISI), and distribution of training on both short-term and long-term habituation were examined. In Experiment 1 (10-s ISI), there appeared to be a floor effect which resulted in a low level of responding regardless of training on Day 1; thus no evidence for long-term habituation after training at a 10-s ISI could be found. In Experiment 2 (60-s ISI), worms that received distributed and massed habituation training with 60 stimuli showed a significantly lower level of responding relative to untrained controls. The distributed habituation training appeared to be more effective at inducing long-term habituation and was used in the subsequent experiments. To characterize the effects of heat shock treatments used in the behavioral experiments that follow, the effects of heat shock on two assays, the induction of a heat shock protein gene, hsp16, and the rate of egg-laying were measured in Experiment 3. All heat shock treatments used caused the induction of hsp16. In addition, the number of eggs laid during a fixed interval after heat shock was sensitive to the heat shock treatments given in Experiments 4 through 8. In Experiments 4 through 8, the effects of heat shock on short- and long-term habituation were examined. Heat shock, which acts as a general cellular stressor, was administered at different times before, during and after training. In Experiment 4, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered, ending 2 h before training on Day 1. Heat shock before training did not affect the initial level of responding on Day 1, habituation during training, short-term retention of habituation between blocks of training or long-term retention of habituation. In Experiment 5, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered during the rest periods of distributed training in the 1-h interval after each training block. While heat shock during training had no significant effect on responding on Day 1, long-term habituation was blocked. In Experiment 6, the possibility that heat shock before training would prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training by inducing thermal tolerance was examined. This was tested by administering heat shock (45 min, 32°C) that ended 2 h before training and heat shock during training. It was found that heat shock before training did not prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training. In Experiment 7, the effect of heat shock that ended 2 h before the retention test on Day 2 on the retention of long-term habituation was examined. It was found that heat shock on Day 2 did not disrupt the retention of habituation. Finally, in Experiment 8, the effect of brief heat shock (15 min, 32°C) at different intervals in the rest period following the training blocks was examined in an attempt to more narrowly define a critical period for consolidation of long-term habituation. Although there was no significant effect of brief heat shock on retention of habituation, the pattern of the data suggests that there may be a period of greater vulnerability worth further investigation. In summary, heat shock given before training or before the retention test did not affect long-term habituation, while heat shock during training disrupted consolidation of long-term habituation. Taken together, these behavioral results provide the foundation for an investigation of the cellular processes underlying long-term memory in C. elegans. By exploring the dynamics of the formation of long-term habituation, intervals of time critical to the formation of long-term habituation were defined. This in turn will help to focus attention on the cellular processes whose activity during those intervals of time may be important to the consolidation of long-term memory.
3

Habit learning in humans acquisition, performance, and interactions with declarative memory /

Foerde, Karin Elaine, January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--UCLA, 2007. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 156).
4

Factors affecting long-term habituation in Caenorhabditis elegans

Beck, Christine Daily O’Brien 11 1900 (has links)
The objective of these experiments was to explore long-term memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. This examination of memory in a simple organism with accessible genetics and a well understood biology may permit later work to define the cellular processes that underlie long-term memory. Habituation training with a vibrational stimulus was administered on Day 1, and the retention test of a block of stimuli was given 24 h after the end of training on Day 2. Long-term retention of habituation was evident as a lower level of responding on Day 2 relative to the level of responding on Day 2 of untrained controls or the initial level of responding of worms on Day 1. In Experiments 1 and 2, a habituation training protocol that produced long-term retention of habituation was established, and the effects of stimulus number, interstimulus interval (ISI), and distribution of training on both short-term and long-term habituation were examined. In Experiment 1 (10-s ISI), there appeared to be a floor effect which resulted in a low level of responding regardless of training on Day 1; thus no evidence for long-term habituation after training at a 10-s ISI could be found. In Experiment 2 (60-s ISI), worms that received distributed and massed habituation training with 60 stimuli showed a significantly lower level of responding relative to untrained controls. The distributed habituation training appeared to be more effective at inducing long-term habituation and was used in the subsequent experiments. To characterize the effects of heat shock treatments used in the behavioral experiments that follow, the effects of heat shock on two assays, the induction of a heat shock protein gene, hsp16, and the rate of egg-laying were measured in Experiment 3. All heat shock treatments used caused the induction of hsp16. In addition, the number of eggs laid during a fixed interval after heat shock was sensitive to the heat shock treatments given in Experiments 4 through 8. In Experiments 4 through 8, the effects of heat shock on short- and long-term habituation were examined. Heat shock, which acts as a general cellular stressor, was administered at different times before, during and after training. In Experiment 4, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered, ending 2 h before training on Day 1. Heat shock before training did not affect the initial level of responding on Day 1, habituation during training, short-term retention of habituation between blocks of training or long-term retention of habituation. In Experiment 5, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered during the rest periods of distributed training in the 1-h interval after each training block. While heat shock during training had no significant effect on responding on Day 1, long-term habituation was blocked. In Experiment 6, the possibility that heat shock before training would prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training by inducing thermal tolerance was examined. This was tested by administering heat shock (45 min, 32°C) that ended 2 h before training and heat shock during training. It was found that heat shock before training did not prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training. In Experiment 7, the effect of heat shock that ended 2 h before the retention test on Day 2 on the retention of long-term habituation was examined. It was found that heat shock on Day 2 did not disrupt the retention of habituation. Finally, in Experiment 8, the effect of brief heat shock (15 min, 32°C) at different intervals in the rest period following the training blocks was examined in an attempt to more narrowly define a critical period for consolidation of long-term habituation. Although there was no significant effect of brief heat shock on retention of habituation, the pattern of the data suggests that there may be a period of greater vulnerability worth further investigation. In summary, heat shock given before training or before the retention test did not affect long-term habituation, while heat shock during training disrupted consolidation of long-term habituation. Taken together, these behavioral results provide the foundation for an investigation of the cellular processes underlying long-term memory in C. elegans. By exploring the dynamics of the formation of long-term habituation, intervals of time critical to the formation of long-term habituation were defined. This in turn will help to focus attention on the cellular processes whose activity during those intervals of time may be important to the consolidation of long-term memory. / Arts, Faculty of / Psychology, Department of / Graduate
5

Newborn longterm retention of speech sounds.

Swain, Irina Uta 01 January 1987 (has links) (PDF)
No description available.
6

The effects of lesions in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and related areas on emotional responses to cigarette smoking

Naqvi, Nasir Hasnain. January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (PH. D.)--University of Iowa, 2007. / Supervisor: Antoine Bechara. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 174-195).
7

Interactions of habituation and sensitization at the network level illustrated by the tentacle withdrawal reflex of a snail

Prescott, Steven A. January 1997 (has links)
A significant goal in studies on learning and memory is to relate cellular plasticity to the modification of behaviour. The phenomenon of dual-process learning affords an ideal opportunity to explore the complexities inherent in establishing this relationship. Dual-process learning occurs when depression (habituation) and facilitation (sensitization) are expressed simultaneously within a neural network and compete to determine the behavioural outcome. A large body of literature is reviewed to define characteristics which are common across the neural networks that exhibit dual-process learning: depression occurs at loci early in the reflex pathway, upstream of the modulatory system necessary for the induction of facilitation. Consequently, depression not only competes directly with facilitation for the determination, of behavioural change (by serial and/or parallel expression), but depression also precludes the ongoing development and maintenance of sensitization (by serial induction). A mathematical model is presented to formally describe the nature of this competition and how this competition leads to the kinetics of dual-process learning. The tentacle withdrawal reflex of the snag Helix aspersa exhibits dual-process learning and was further investigated in this study. The neural circuit mediating tentacle withdrawal is described along with the nature and the location of plasticity which occurs within that circuit. In turn, plasticity at the cellular level is related, via the network level, to plasticity at the behavioural level. The data demonstrate the importance of localizing the sites of plasticity within a neural network in order to explain (1) how plasticity at a particular locus influences plasticity occurring elsewhere in the network and (2) how plasticity at different loci affect different aspects of behaviour.
8

The effect of d-amphetamine on habituation of schedule controlled operant behavior

Packer, Robert R., January 2008 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (M.S. in psychology)--Washington State University, August 2008. / Includes bibliographical references (p. 19-23).
9

The role of the hippocampus and matrix metaloproteinases on habituation of the head-shake response task/classical conditioning paradigm

Wiediger, Roberta V., January 2008 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Washington State University, December 2008. / Title from PDF title page (viewed on July 10, 2009). "Department of Psychology." Includes bibliographical references.
10

Within-session changes in responding during simulated slot machine play

Sutich, Daniel H. January 2008 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--University of Nevada, Reno, 2008. / "December, 2008." Includes bibliographical references (leaves 30-35). Online version available on the World Wide Web.

Page generated in 0.1061 seconds