Spelling suggestions: "subject:"darkestafrica.""
1 |
On the behavioural ecology and vocal communication of the brown-headed parrot (Poicephalus cryptoxanthus)Taylor, Stuart. 13 December 2013 (has links)
The Brown-headed Parrot (Poicephalus cryptoxanthus) is a poorly
known species inhabiting open woodland in south-eastern Africa. This study
elucidates critical aspects of the species ecology and although each of these
categories impinge on one another, it concentrates on two broad biological
aspects, diet and breeding biology, and vocalizations.
The species has a generalist diet, switching from one suite of food
species to another as and when those species become available, with no
species critical for its survival. Analysis of dietary items throughout the year
and comparison with handling times and availability reveals that at no time is
the species under dietary constraint.
Evidence from association indices and behavioural observation shows
that the popular view that the Brown-headed Parrot forms pairs just before
breeding is erroneous. Birds retain pair bonds and the bond is long-term,
lasting at least throughout the year. Congregations are therefore of a classical
fission/fusion type with the sub-units being the paired males and females.
A mathematical model of the growth of captive chicks is presented, as a
guideline to alert potential breeders of Brown-headed Parrots of malnutrition
or disease. The species is a secondary cavity hole nester and whilst, the
breeding biology of the species is summarised, the importance of large and old
trees for breeding opportunities of the species is emphasised. This theme is
continued by testing various adaptive hatching hypotheses as possible
explanations of asynchronous hatching in the species. It is suggested that
asynchronous hatching may be an adaptive strategy moderating against the
number of suitable nesting cavities.
The vocalization repertoire of the Brown-headed Parrot is described
and seven separate vocalizations are recognized. None of these are associated
with sexual situations, offering further evidence of a long-term pair bond.
Evidence is offered that Brown-headed Parrot chicks can recognise
their parents from individual vocal signatures supporting previous evidence
from a number of species where chicks may mingle with unrelated chicks.
Conversely, parents seem to be unable to recognise their chicks in the same
way. It is concluded that this inability may be a result of strong one-way selection pressure, where the costs outweigh the benefits for parents with
more than one chick or may be related to the experimental design.
Individual recognition by voice implies individual voice differences and
the adult double chip contact call is analysed using multivariate statistical
techniques. The analysis separates individuals on the basis of frequency and
temporal patterns and it is concluded that these parameters may allow
individual voice differentiation.
Finally, high frequency aspects of the double chip contact call are
examined. These frequencies lie above the normally accepted upper threshold
of avian hearing. From laboratory and field experiments, behavioural evidence
is presented suggesting that the Brown-headed Parrot reacts to these
frequencies and may use their degradation as a means of ranging distances to
conspecifics. / Thesis (Ph.D.)-University of Natal, Pietermaritzburg, 2002.
|
2 |
Biology of the greyheaded parrot Poicephalus fuscicollis suahelicus Reichnow.Symes, Craig Thomas. 30 October 2013 (has links)
This study was conducted to investigate the biology of the Greyheaded Parrot Poicephalus fuscicollis suahelicus in the wild. Field work was conducted in north-east South Africa in the southern limit of the range of the Greyheaded Parrot during two field seasons. Observations from
August to December 1999 in the Levubu region, south of the Soutpansberg mountain range, included months of the non-breeding season when Greyheaded Parrots occur seasonally in the area. Observations in the Luvhuvhu-Mutale river confluence area from March to August 2000
included months of the breeding season. No field work was conducted from January to March 2000 due to exceptionally high rains in the southern African sub-region, that prevented access to sites. The Greyheaded Parrot has a widespread distribution, through southern, south Central and
East Africa, that has possible changed little in recent years. However, local populations are likely to have suffered extinctions due to habitat destruction and capture for the illegal trade. Populations in protected areas (Makuya Park and Kruger National Park) are less vulnerable to capture than unprotected areas. The conservation of the Greyheaded Parrot outside of protected areas is highlighted. In various parts of the range of the Greyheaded Parrot seasonal movements occur in response to food and nest site availability. Nest sites are possibly limiting in certain parts of its
range due to habitat destruction. During post-breeding flocking, the occurrence of larger flocks, possibly family units (mean ± S.E = 4.7 ± 0.2), is common, when birds wander in search for seasonally available food sources. During this period density of Greyheaded Parrots is 0.28
birds/100 ha. Monogamous pairs are more conspicuous during the breeding season (mean ± S.E = 2.1 ± 0.1) and density estimates are 0.14 birds/100 ha. Egg laying is synchronous between pairs with the timed appearance and flocking of juvenile flocks in spring (August/September). Breeding during the dry season reduces competition with other large cavity nesting bird species. Observations suggest that a skewed sex ratio exists in the population (males:females = 2:1). Daily movements are characterised by a bimodal activity pattern. Early morning movements involve flights to activity centres where the accumulation of numerous smaller flocks occur. Here preening, allo-preening and socializing occur with drinking and/or feeding occurring if food and/or water are available. Thereafter, birds move to regular feeding sites, to feed. Activity is decreased during the heat of the day with birds sleeping, resting and/or preening in the canopies of trees. Late afternoon activities involve increased levels of activity and late afternoon return flights to roosts.
Greyheaded Parrots are specialist feeders, accessing the kernel of predominantly unripe fruit. This feeding strategy reduces competition with other frugivore competitors. During any one time few tree species are fed on by the Greyheaded Parrot. Greyheaded Parrots were also observed feeding on bark in the breeding season. Two, almost fully fledged, chicks were found dead in a nest and the causes of death undetermined. Their crops contained numerous pieces of masticated bark and insect parts.
Behaviours and vocalizations of the Greyheaded Parrot were similar to that recorded in the Cape Parrot. Recognition of the Greyheaded Parrot as a separate species based on species specific calls and DNA warrants further investigation. / Thesis (M.Sc.)-University of Natal, Pietermaritzburg, 2001.
|
3 |
Comparative aspects of the thermal biology of African and Australian parrots.Burton, Stephen Leslie. January 2006 (has links)
Deserticolous birds inhabit an environment characterised by high ambient temperatures and low
rainfall that has low primary productivity. The combination of these factors may lead to the
evolution of adaptations that minimise food and water requirements. One physiological
adaptation that has been found in many deserticolous birds is the reduction of basal metabolic
rate (BMR).
I measured metabolic rate in the laboratory using four species of African lovebirds
(Agapornis) , and four species of Australian grass parakeets (one Neopsephotus and three
Neophema), all similar in body mass. Tests for differences between groups were carried out
using both conventional and phylogenetically independent methods. The BMRs of the lovebird
and grass parakeet species were not statistically correlated with habitat type. These results
confirm the findings of previous studies on the effect of desert conditions on the BMR of parrots.
I also found no significant differences in BMR between the species assemblages from different
continents. The lack of significant differences in BMR between deserticolous and nondeserticolous
parrots supports the idea that birds are "ex-adapted" to living in desert
environments. I suggest that the results may have been affected by phenotypic plasticity in BMR,
as recent evidence has shown that the scaling exponent of BMR differs between captive-raised
and wild-caught birds.
To elucidate the effect of origin (captive-raised vs. wild-caught) on the BMR of birds
used in this study a large scale analysis of bird BMR data was undertaken. BMR and body mass
data for 242 species of birds were obtained from the literature, this study, and unpublished data
from various sources. A phylogeny was constructed using molecular and morphological
phylogenies from the literature, and analysed using conventional and phylogenetically
independent methods. The conventional analysis found significant differences in the scaling
exponents of BMR of captive-raised and wild-caught birds. However the phylogenetically
independent method showed non-significant differences between these two groups. Conventional
analysis of differences between parrots and all other birds yielded significant differences
between these two groups, with parrots having significantly higher BMRs than other birds.
Again the phylogenetically independent analysis found non-significant differences between these
VII
two groups. A test of homogeneity of variance between these two groups found significant
differences between the variances ofthe two groups, probably due to disparity in sample size and
range of body sizes. The conventional and phylogenetically independent tests for differences
between captive-raised and wild-caught parrots yielded non-significant results, suggesting that
the parrots are not subject to the phenotypic adjustments postulated for all other birds.
The lack of significant differences between captive-raised and wild-caught parrots
suggests that the analyses of differing habitat type for African and Australian parrots is indeed
valid. / Thesis (M.Sc.)-University of KwaZulu-Natal, Pietermaritzburg, 2006.
|
Page generated in 0.0418 seconds