Spelling suggestions: "subject:"mpsychology, cognitive."" "subject:"mpsychology, aognitive.""
31 |
The recall of multiple stimulus attributes in four- to six-item listsChmielewski, Cynthia Elizabeth January 1999 (has links)
Three experiments are reported which investigate short-term memory for multidimensional stimuli; in particular, item information, spatial location, and temporal order. The three experiments differ in list length, which was increased to eliminate ceiling effects, which may have skewed the results of previous studies. All three dimensions were varied independently, and, after stimulus presentation, when one was presented as a cue, the other two dimensions were to be recalled. The results suggest that temporal order and item information are highly associated in short-term memory, while spatial location and item information are not highly associated. The relationship between spatial location and temporal order is more complex, with list length and attributes of individual stimuli having an effect.
|
32 |
Perceptual organization of random dot patterns: Area salient and memorable, proximity salient but forgottenJewell, Stephen W. January 2004 (has links)
I report four experiments designed to investigate the perceptual grouping processes by better understanding how a simple pattern of dots comes to be represented in the visual memory systems. Subjects were briefly shown a random set of point-like dots which were then masked, and subsequently reappeared with a possible added or deleted dot. Subjects detected deletions more accurately than additions and detected changes to the most peripheral dots more reliably than changes to interior dots, even those much closer to fixation. The most peripheral points appear to be objectized in a process that might be analogized as shrink wrapping. The circumscribed area is memorable and easily recalled despite brief presentations. The data are consistent with the view that the fastest of the perceptual processes is a global system which proceeds from the periphery inward toward fixation and that a separate slower local analytical sequence starts at fixation and works outward. I conjecture that proximity, one, if not the most salient of all features in the early perceptual processes that extract data from the sensory store and encode it in the visual memory systems, is not itself stored as part of the representation.
|
33 |
Proactive interference in phonological and semantic short-term memory deficitsHamilton, Andrew Cris January 2004 (has links)
These experiments explore the role of proactive interference in short-term memory using a probe recognition task (Monsell, 1974). Experiment 1 indicated that such tasks were particularly difficult for certain patients. Experiment 2 employed negative probes that were phonologically or semantically related to list items in the same or previous list. Subjects included patients with semantic and phonological short-term memory deficits, older adults and undergraduate subjects. Healthy subjects showed significant interference effects on both phonologically and semantically related probes, but only for probes related to items in the same list. A patient with a semantic short-term memory deficit showed interference effects for both phonologically and semantically related probes in both same and previous list conditions. A patient with a phonological short-term memory deficit showed no such effect. Results are discussed in context of theoretical implications for short-term memory models proposing distinctions between phonological and semantic capacities.
|
34 |
Exploring executive functions in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using event related potentialsKothmann, Delia Katherine January 2006 (has links)
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a prevalent neurobehavioral disorder that affects individuals in early childhood and is characterized by cognitive impairments associated with executive functioning. However, the exact nature of the impairment(s) is/are unclear. In a recent meta-analysis, Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter, and Wager (2000) demonstrated that there are at least three separable frontal executive functions: set switching, working memory updating, and response inhibition. Here we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate whether the cognitive impairments seen in ADHD are specific to one of these executive functions or rather represent a global executive functioning deficiency. The current studies examined the time course and scalp localization of executive functions in Combined Type ADHD and in comparison control children by implementing modified versions of three executive tasks used by Miyake et al. (2000), local-global, tone-monitoring, and Stroop; versions more appropriate for the ADHD population. An additional non-executive task (visual oddball) was included in order to demonstrate that the deficits in ADHD are specific to executive functioning. The goal was to determine if children with ADHD perform differently on these three executive tasks and if this difference can be attributed to a deficit associated with one or more of the executive functions. The current findings show that ADHD impacts only a subset of cognitive operations in the executive functions, leaving the other ERPs in the executive tasks and the visual oddball unaffected. Specifically, the ADHD group was impaired on tasks involving allocation of attention and response inhibition, the cognitive operations most closely related to the diagnostic criteria for this subtype of ADHD. These findings may extend our knowledge of the time course and localization of executive functions and provide a tool for studying the nature of disrupted executive functioning in ADHD.
|
35 |
Impulsivity and neural systems of rewardsMartin, Laura E. January 2006 (has links)
Behavioral studies of decision-making in impulsive individuals demonstrate biases for immediate rewards. Decision-making includes evaluating motivational values of both options and actions. In the current studies, event related potentials (ERPs) and functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) were used to assess the temporal and spatial properties of the ventral tegmental (VTA) dopamine (DA) reward system during item evaluation and action/outcome-monitoring in impulsivity. A passive reward evaluation and an active action/outcome-monitoring task were used to test a model predicting that dysregulation of the VTA DA reward system leads to overestimations of reward value in the prefrontal cortex and underestimations of punishments in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) during reward evaluation and action/outcome-monitoring respectively. Passive reward evaluation ERP results showed a prefrontal P2a ERP index of reward expectation that did not differ with impulsivity. FMRI reward evaluation results indicated that the basal ganglia and left superior frontal cortex were sensitive to rewarding outcomes, whereas the medial ACC was sensitive to non-rewarding outcomes. Left superior frontal cortex fMRI activation showed greater differentiation between better than expected and worse than expected outcomes among high impulsive participants. Action/outcome-monitoring ERP results showed an ERN ERP index the ACC in the action/outcome-monitoring with the greatest response on error trials that led to punishments. The ERN was also larger among high impulsives, compared to the low impulsives, when errors led to missed rewards. The fMRI results showed that the basal ganglia responded like the ERN to errors resulting in punishments. The ACC and right middle frontal regions showed greater activation to correct responses, and a right ACC region showed larger activations when correct responses led to rewards. However, these activations did not differ with impulsivity. In conclusion, the current results suggest differential responsiveness of the mesotelencephalic reward system in impulsive individuals which may lead to reward hypersensitivity but not punishment hyposensitivity during item evaluation and action/outcome-monitoring among high impulsive participants, as seen in the left superior frontal fMRI activation during item evaluation and in the ACC ERP activity during outcome-monitoring. Differences between the ERP and fMRI results may reflect the different physiological processes that give rise to the signals.
|
36 |
Refining theoretical models of visual sampling in supervisory control tasks: Examining the influence of alarm frequency, effort, value, and salienceFleetwood, Michael D. January 2005 (has links)
This work is concerned with examining in a formal quantitative manner what human observers look at and what the objects of their gaze tell them. Three models designed to describe and predict the allocation of human attention in supervisory control tasks were investigated. A series of three experiments examined the relative influence of five factors on the sampling patterns of participants: the information generation rate of the information signal (bandwidth), the frequency of significant, i.e., task relevant, events on an information source (alarm frequency), the payoff matrix associated with missing or detecting critical events (value), the visual salience of the events, and the cost of making an observation. The paradigm employed is similar to that developed by Senders and colleagues (1964), in which observers were asked to monitor an array of four simulated ammeters and to press a button whenever the pointer of any ammeter entered an "alarm zone." Aspects of three mathematical models, Senders's constrained random sampler, Wickens and colleagues SEEV model, and Pirolli's and Card's Information Foraging Theory Model, were combined to form seven different models predicting performance in the task. The sampling patterns predicted by each model were compared against the eye movement data of participants. Results of the three experiments indicate that participants' sampling patterns were sensitive to the experimental manipulations. Comparisons of the model predicted patterns of attention allocation to those in the participant data indicated that different models described different participants. Participants who performed poorly at the task were best described by models incorporating bandwidth. Participants who performed well at the task were best described by models incorporating alarm frequency, and those who performed best at the task were not well-described by any of the models. Overall the models based on Information Foraging Theory were the most robust in predicting the attention allocation patterns of participants. Implications of each of the experimental manipulations and of the fit of the models to the participant data are discussed.
|
37 |
An investigation of semantic and associative relatedness and semantic short-term memory in speech productionBiegler, Kelly Ann January 2004 (has links)
Eight experiments investigated the role of semantic and associative relatedness and short-term memory in speech planning. Subjects produced a conjoined noun phrase corresponding to picture pairs that were semantically related only, semantically and associatively related, and associatively related only. Interference effects, i.e., longer onset latencies were observed for neurologically intact undergraduates and elderly controls in both semantic conditions but were not apparent in the associative only condition. Interference effects only occurred under the following conditions: (1) The degree of semantic relatedness was high between picture pairs. (2) Each picture was presented several times. (3) Related and unrelated matched pairs occurred in the same block and (4) Several pairs appeared from the same category. A phonological STM patient showed interference effects for semantically related pairs within the range of elderly controls. However, a semantic STM patient showed mixed results across several experiments. His inconsistent performance pattern was attributed, in part, to long term priming effects resulting from repeated testing with the same materials. Overall, the results suggest that the lexical-semantic information for both pictures was planned prior to production, and that semantic and associative relatedness differentially affect planning in speech production.
|
38 |
Specificity of transfer-appropriate processing in indirect memoryHuang, Yanliu January 2004 (has links)
Studies of hyperspecific transfer of processing in indirect memory tests are reviewed. A procedure for deriving a comprehensive assessment of priming of indirect memory is then proposed. The procedure is illustrated in Experiment 1, in which prior study of randomly selected words presented with no item-specific context and what could be construed as neutral instructions primed their identification more in a perceptual (fragment completion) task than in a conceptual (semantic cuing) task. Experiments 2 and 3 failed to provide evidence for hyperspecific (i.e., sublexical) transfer of processing in an indirect memory task that called for rapid identification of gradually presented words. Experiment 4 also failed to provide evidence of hyperspecific transfer of processing, despite following more closely the procedure of an experiment (Hayman & Tulving, 1989, Experiment 4) that has provided such evidence. It appears that hyperspecific transfer of processing may be more elusive than sometimes assumed.
|
39 |
Selective remembering in an orienting-task paradigmCampbell, Madeline January 2004 (has links)
The levels-of-processing effect is extraordinarily, even puzzlingly, robust. It occurs even when a memory test is expected, ample study time is given, and deep processing is encouraged regardless of orienting task. Thus, processing appears to be "fixed" by the requirements of the orienting task. This enigma is explored in a selective remembering procedure involving the recall of words of arbitrarily varying values. After verifying that selectivity is substantially localized at the encoding rather than the recall stage of the remembering process (Experiment 1), recall was found to be selective despite the imposition of an orienting task and regardless of whether item values were assigned according to the items' semantic category (Experiment 2) or entirely at random (Experiment 3). Indeed, no evidence was found for any effect at all of orienting tasks on selectivity (Experiment 4). Orienting tasks do not, after all, universally dominate the encoding process.
|
40 |
The search for emergent features in vision: Looking at configural superiority effects with the odd-quadrant taskPortillo, Mary C. January 2006 (has links)
A series of three experiments were designed to test for Emergent Features, which, in Gestalt psychology, are indicative of grouping. By using basic elements such as dots or lines and building EFs sequentially, while maintaining the original discrimination constant, it was possible to compare the strength of different EFs. Proximity, Orientation, Symmetry, Linearity, Surroundedness, Inside/Outside relationship, Collinearity, Parallelism, Inflection Point and Closure were tested. A Configural Superiority Effect (CSE, where the RT in a composite condition is shorter than the RT in a singleton condition) was diagnostic of the presence of an EF in an Odd Quadrant task. Proximity, Orientation, Linearity, Inside/Outside and Collinearity produced CSEs and thus behave as basic in human vision. Mixed results were obtained for Symmetry and Parallelism. Further, Inflection Point and Closure were only suggested in the stimuli, so they cannot be ruled out as EFs.
|
Page generated in 0.0737 seconds