Spelling suggestions: "subject:"1oading ability"" "subject:"dhading ability""
21 |
The validity and applicability of two modified cloze procedures (beginning of the page procedure and "instant" beginning of the page procedure) measured against the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test and equated with the cloze procedure and Fry Readability GraphParkinson, Dianne January 1980 (has links)
This correlational study examined the Beginning of the Page Procedure (B.O.P.P) and the "instant" Beginning of the Page Procedure as measures for assessing readability: One hundred ninety-six grade nine students (106 male and 90 female) took part in the study and their scores on the cloze procedure, the B.O.P.P. and the "instant" B.O.P.P. were correlated with the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test Form A - Blue Level (hereafter referred to as the Stanford Diagnostic ). The Stanford Diagnostic was used as the anchor test and the students were randomly assigned to each of the three groups; Analyses included the calculation of means associated with the Stanford Diagnostic scores for each subgroup, and analysis of the variance between sexes within each subgroup. An equivalency table is provided which estimates the Stanford Diagnostic scores for a given cloze procedure, B.O.P.P. or "instant" B.O.P.P. score; Using the Stanford Diagnostic grade
score equivalent to 40 percent on the cloze procedure, the readability level of the passage was determined. This was then compared to the readability level estimated by the Fry Graph.
Respective correlations of .53 and .67 were found between the B.O.P.P. and "instant" B.O.P.P. with the Stanford Diagnostic suggesting both are good indicators of the students' ability to handle the given passage. Similarly the Fry Graph and the Stanford Diagnostic, grade score equal to 40 percent on the cloze procedure, found the passages to be at virtually the same level of difficulty. All results however, were limited to the passage studied and should not be generalized to other materials.
When a significance level of .05 was used no significant difference was found between the male and female performance levels on any of the tests administered. / Education, Faculty of / Educational Studies (EDST), Department of / Graduate
|
22 |
Encoding and verification by children at three levels of reading comprehension proficiencyStamm, Sharon Winston. January 1978 (has links)
Note:
|
23 |
A Comparative Analysis of Reading Habits and Abilities of Students in Selected Elementary Schools in North Louisiana with and without Centralized LibrariesLowe, Joy L. 08 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
24 |
The process of learning to read and the Test of Early Reading Ability (TERA)Wong, Terry, n/a January 1983 (has links)
n/a
|
25 |
The phonological mediation hypothesis evidence from Chinese students with hearing impairment張嘉恩, Cheung, Ka-yan, Winnie. January 2011 (has links)
In Western countries, there are around 8-10% of school age children suffering
from mild to moderate reading difficulties. Similarly, in Hong Kong the prevalence
of reading difficulties among school age children was found to be 9.7- 12%. An
understanding of the mechanisms involved in reading helps us to determine the skills
necessary for successful reading.
The Phonological Mediation Hypothesis (PMH), which claims that
phonological recoding is a necessary phase during lexical access, is widely known
for its postulation that phonological awareness would be a significant prognostic
indicator of reading development. Theoretically, individuals who have difficulties in
phonological recoding during lexical access should also encounter reading
difficulties. Good readers with hearing impairment are, therefore, considered as
counter examples of PMH.
If the above prediction of PHM is true, skilled readers with hearing impairment
should have the ability to develop an intact phonological representation and hence
are more capable of phonological recoding. In this study, the reading behaviors of
children with hearing impairment (HI), that of their reading level matched (RL) and
that of their chronological age matched (CA) controls were compared in three
tasks—an auditory perceptual task of onset rime awareness (TAPOR); a synonym
decision task (SDT); and a homophone decision task (HDT). The results for TAPOR
showed that auditory perceptual ability (APOR) accounted for 49% of the variance in
the reading ability of children with hearing impairment. In addition, results of cross
group comparisons on the scores in TAPOR demonstrated a possible causal
relationship between APOR and reading ability in subjects with hearing impairment.
On the other hand, SDT and HDT results indicated a significant preference for
orthographic foils in RL and HI subjects with low reading ability. An increasing
tendency to choose synonyms or homophones, and a decreasing tendency to use
orthographic distractors was observed across subject groups with Primary 1, Primary
2 and Primary 6 reading abilities. A similar but delayed pattern of change in
preference for distractors was observed in HI subjects. The results only partially
agree with PMH. An alternative hypothesis—the Psycholinguistic Grain Size
Theory (PGST) — might be a better model to explain the observed results. With
reference to the results of TAPOR, the correlation between reading ability and
auditory perceptual ability could be explained in terms of the ‘availability problem’
postulated in this latter model. In the same way, the early emergence of orthographic
effect in almost all subjects except CA controls and the late emergence of a number
of reading strategies operating at different levels of grain size can be better explained
by ‘consistency problems’ and ‘granularity problems’ proposed in PGST. These
findings are considered, and directions for further studies are outlined. / published_or_final_version / Speech and Hearing Sciences / Master / Master of Philosophy
|
26 |
Gender differences of reading ability in Chinese childrenJia, Fanlu., 贾凡路. January 2013 (has links)
Generally, females are regarded as the group who has better verbal and reading abilities (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Denno, 1982), although there are still some disagreements on particular aspects or language systems (Hyde & Linn, 1988; Hetherington & Parke, 1986). We asked whether gender differences of reading ability exist in Chinese children and if so, how the difference may be demonstrated. In addition, we investigated whether the development of reading ability follows a hierarchical model (Gough &Hillinger, 1980; Ehri, 1980; Ehri & Wilce, 1985), according to which, the gender differences may vary with age and reading experience.
We tested children’s character reading ability and phonological awareness in a Shandong primary school. Fifty-five second graders and forty-three fourth graders performed behavioral tasks containing a Chinese character reading test, a Chinese onset-rime oddity test and an English onset-rime oddity test.
We found that Chinese children exhibited a significant gender difference in Chinese character reading ability. However, we have not found gender differences on onset-rime level phonological awareness. Thus, gender differences of reading ability exist in Chinese children, even if the differences only cover partial phonological or orthographic skills.
The present data have also yielded the predicted result that the development of gender differences change with age and reading experience. Specifically, the gender difference on Chinese character reading ability emerged after Grade 2. In terms of the onset-rime oddity task, the performances of boys and girls showed no striking difference at any grades.
These results reveal that gender differences of reading ability exist in Chinese children, especially in children in higher grades (Grade 4). Different types of verbal skills and learning attitude should be taken into account, and be examined in the future. / published_or_final_version / Linguistics / Master / Master of Arts
|
27 |
Modeling ESP ability in reading : a focus on interaction among grammatical knowledge, background knowledge and strategic competenceCai, Yuyang, 蔡雨阳 January 2013 (has links)
abstract / Education / Doctoral / Doctor of Philosophy
|
28 |
THE RELATIONSHIP OF SELECTED ORAL LANGUAGE COMPONENTS TO READING ACHIEVEMENT OF THIRD-GRADE STUDENTSOlson, Patricia Hagey, 1926- January 1976 (has links)
No description available.
|
29 |
THE SCORE PRECISION OF AN ITEM TYPE TO TEST PHONIC AND CONTEXT SKILLS SEPARATELY AND CONJOINTLYGarcia, Cara Lynne, 1943- January 1977 (has links)
No description available.
|
30 |
A comparison of curriculum-based and norm-referenced measures in the identification of reading difficultyDunn, Rita L. 05 1900 (has links)
The purpose of this study is to investigate the
technical adequacy of two reading Curriculum—Based Measures
(CBM5), to examine the relationships of the CBM5 to normreferenced
tests, and to compare the strength of
relationship of both kinds of measures to school—based
indices of reading performance. The two CBMs (a word list
sampled from several reading series and a passage reading
test composed of ten Ginn 720 passages) were taken from the
literature; comprehensive information about their technical
adequacy had not been previously available.
A review of the literature indicates that CBM,
particularly reading CBM, is gaining increased attention in
education because of claims regarding its utility in
monitoring pupil progress, its ease of administration, and
its relationship to local curricula as well as to learning
gains. This study examined how reading CBMs and two
subtests from the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement
(Kaufman & Kaufman, 1985) relate to each other and to three
school—based indices of reading performance: a school
district reading test, program placement status (learning
disabled or regular education), and a teacher rating scale
of reading skill. Grade four students from one metropolitan Vancouver
school district served as subjects (n=105). Of these, 35
were classified as learning disabled and 70 were classified
as regular education. Learning disabled status was
determined by judgments of a school district screening
committee and by examining previous psychoeducational
assessments.
Reliability indices calculated on the CBMs supported
claims for technical adequacy. These estimates were as
follows: internal consistency of the word list was .97,
internal consistency of the reading passages was .98 and
.94 for reading speed and accuracy, test—retest
reliability of the reading passages was .89 and .79 for
reading speed and accuracy, and inter—rater reliability of
the reading passages was .99 and .96 for reading speed and
accuracy. Results indicated that the CBMs used in this
study have high reliabilities.
CBM5, especially the speed score from the- reading
passages, demonstrated strong relationships to the two
norm—referenced subtests. The pattern of correlations
between the measures differed between the learning disabled
and normal sample; analyses of variance demonstrated that
all measures used in the study discriminated between the
learning disabled and the regular education groups. Stepwise multiple regression and canonical analysis
indicated that the two norm—referenced subtests, the speed
score from the Curriculum—Based Reading Passages, and the
accuracy score from the Curriculum—Based Word List were
most efficient in “predicting” the three school-based
indices of reading performance. Evidence for concurrent
validity of curriculum—based and norm—referenced measures
was found in this study. When administration time,
instructional utility, and technical properties are
considered, results indicated that the Kaufman Test of
Educational Achievement Reading Decoding subtest and the
Curriculum—Based Reading Passages speed score are the most
efficient of the predictor measures investigated in
identifying and programming for Year Four children with
significant reading difficulty. Implications for further
research and the potential of CBM to accommodate
instructional and measurement needs is discussed.
|
Page generated in 0.0729 seconds