• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 4592
  • 2936
  • 962
  • 544
  • 462
  • 450
  • 408
  • 94
  • 91
  • 91
  • 91
  • 91
  • 91
  • 90
  • 88
  • Tagged with
  • 13090
  • 1753
  • 1641
  • 1240
  • 1164
  • 1162
  • 979
  • 868
  • 706
  • 695
  • 686
  • 677
  • 640
  • 617
  • 587
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
21

Teaching for visual literacy critically deconstructing the visual within a democratic education /

Golubieski, Mary R. January 2003 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Miami University, Dept. of Educational Leadership, 2003. / Title from first page of PDF document. Document formatted into pages; contains xv, 316 p. Includes bibliographical references (p. 265-280).
22

Human visual system's selective sensitivity to a rate of change of spatial frequency

Lee, Yunjo. January 2001 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--York University, 2001. Graduate Programme in Psychology. / Typescript. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 95-103). Also available on the Internet. MODE OF ACCESS via web browser by entering the following URL: http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/yorku/fullcit?pMQ66391.
23

291: visual poetry of the proto-Dada avant-garde

Tomfohrde, Carmen Sue. January 2011 (has links)
published_or_final_version / English Studies / Master / Master of Arts
24

Can optic flow recalibrate the perceived straight ahead in the visual control of steering toward a goal?

Siu, Wai-fung, 蕭蔚鋒 January 2013 (has links)
There has long been a debate on which of the following two strategies we adopt while we are steering towards a stationary target: (1) The optic flow strategy, which suggests that we can find our ways towards our target by the alignment of our visually perceived heading, our instantaneous direction of locomotion, with the target. (2) The perceived direction strategy, in which we move towards a target by aligning our perceived straight ahead with the target. As we typically walk straight forward in our daily lives instead of making crab movements, our heading is usually well aligned with our perceived straight ahead while we are walking and it is difficult to tell which of the two strategies a person adopts when we observe him or her walking in naturalistic settings. There are multiple methods to displace the visual information about heading from our perceived straight ahead during walking, but inconsistent results have been obtained with these different methods – some showed that only the perceived direction strategy is used, while some found signs that both strategies are used simultaneously. Researchers supporting the perceived direction strategy suggested that the deviation of participants’ locomotive paths from that predicted by the perceived direction strategy observed in some experiments could be due to participants’ shifts in their perceived straight ahead driven by the displaced heading paradigm. So far, little work that addresses this concern have been conducted, thus, this thesis is intended to investigate this issue. In experiment I, participants were required to steer in virtual environments under displaced optic flow, with their steering performance as well as their shifts in perceived straight ahead at the end of each trial measured. A shift of the perceived straight ahead was observed, and its magnitude increased in conditions with richer optic flow. However, after taking this shift into account, participants still steered on paths intermediate between that predicted by the two different strategies. In experiment II, we have shown that a long period of steering under displaced heading can drive adaptations in our perceived straight ahead and corresponding changes in our steering behavior. From the results we conclude that optic flow has a profound effect on our perception of perceived straight ahead, and on top of that effect, optic flow also directly influences our guidance of steering towards a target. / published_or_final_version / Psychology / Master / Master of Philosophy
25

Effects of binocular fusion and binocular rivalry on cortically evoked potentials

Martin, James Ingram, 1944- January 1969 (has links)
No description available.
26

The effects of a border on the reaction time to the onset and the termination of stimulation in the fovea

Versteeg, Arlen Dale, 1941- January 1970 (has links)
No description available.
27

The relationships among perceived contrast, noise, and content in printed images /

Parush, Avraham. January 1980 (has links)
No description available.
28

The effect of satiation on discriminating horizontal and vertical grids.

Backus, David H. January 1966 (has links)
No description available.
29

The effect of prior experience on apparent movement.

Raskin, Larry Marvin. January 1966 (has links)
The history of apparent movement begins in the 1820's (Boring, 1942), but its full importance for psychology was not recognized until the publication of Wertheimer's paper, Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung, in 1912 (translated in greater part in Shipley, 1961). Wertheimer saw the significance of the tact that, under certain temporal conditions, the successive presentation of a pair of stationary visual abjects "at a considerable spatial distance from one another," evokes the perception of movement. He called this impression of motion in the spatial interval between the two abjects the phi-phenomenon. [...]
30

Cross-modal facilitation of spatial frequency discriminations through auditory frequency cue presentations

Elias, Bartholomew 12 1900 (has links)
No description available.

Page generated in 0.0451 seconds