• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 36
  • 15
  • 9
  • 6
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 95
  • 22
  • 17
  • 17
  • 16
  • 15
  • 13
  • 13
  • 11
  • 11
  • 10
  • 10
  • 10
  • 9
  • 9
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Studien über Seelenvorstellungen in Ozeanien

Fischer, Hans, January 1965 (has links)
Habilitationsscrift--Tübingen. / Bibliography: p. 405-431.
2

The nature of the life concept across the life span

Sheehan, Nancy Welburn, January 1974 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison, 1973. / eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references.
3

Animistic thinking in children

Stern, Harris Weil 01 February 2017 (has links)
The objectives of the study were based on constructs which were originally described and studied by Piaget and some of which were studied subsequently by other authors with contradictory results. The four major objectives of the study were: 1. to reexamine the development of children's concepts of life, and, in particular to systematically investigate the relationship between children's errors in classifying items as alive or not alive and their use of different justifications for those classifications , 2. to attempt to elicit precausal explanations from children in response to demonstration items (Piaget ; originally studied precausality in terms of natural objects and events and subsequent experimenters failed to find the precausal forms for demonstrations). 3. to test the hypothesis that children who give pre- causal explanations will have difficulty in learning a causal relationship, even in the face of repeated experience. 4. to test the hypothesis that children who classify inanimate objects as alive (and are hence, animistic) will be the children who also give the greatest number of precausal explanations for demonstrations. 5. to attempt to relate systematic animism and pre- causality to a standardized measure of cognitive development.' In order to study these constructs and the relationships in between them, 96 children between the ages of four and ten years were individually administered a test battery consisting of (1) an animistic questionnaire, consisting of 21 plant, object, and animal items to be classified as alive or not alive; (2) eight demonstrations about which the children were questioned in order to obtain their explanations for what took place; (3) a causal learning task, requiring the children to isolate a particular cause for the outcome of an event, given a number of trials and some directly relevant, extra experience and (4) the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. The major findings were: 1. that reduction of animism in children is associated with the identification of life with animals' and their characteristics. This association leads children to classify plants as well as objects as not alive, since plants have none of the more obvious characteristics of animals (locomotion, sensation, vocalization), and, it is only at some later stage, when life is identified with more general characteristics (need for air, water, food; death, birth, reproduction), that plants are again classified as alive. 2. Young children do indeed give precausal, non-mechanical explanations for demonstrations. The study suggests that Piaget's particular categories of precausal thought may not have universal validity for all kinds of events or for all children, but that the general characteristics of these explanations which he described (lack of attention to details of how things happen, lack of understanding of temporal sequences of events, and the lack of understanding of the need for spatial contact for the transfer of energy and motion) are found in the explanations of many young children, even for demonstration and mechanical events. 3. Children who gave precausal explanations for the causal learning task did fail to learn the correct cause-effect relationship. 4. There was no support for Piaget's theory that animism, or the attribution of life to objects, has a direct relationship to precausal explanations. In the present study, animistic children were not more likely to use precausal explanations than were non-animistic children. / This thesis was digitized as part of a project begun in 2014 to increase the number of Duke psychology theses available online. The digitization project was spearheaded by Ciara Healy.
4

Animistic beliefs in Malaya

Allard, E. M. A. A. J. January 1941 (has links)
No description available.
5

Rester av primitiv religion bland Värmlands finnbefolkning, jämförda med motsvarande företeelser på svenskt område ...

Palmqvist, Johan Viktor. January 1924 (has links)
Akademisk avhandling--Lund. / "Källor": p. [194]-198.
6

The occurrence of animistic thinking as a function of sentence context and set factors.

Simmons, Alvin Joseph 01 January 1954 (has links) (PDF)
No description available.
7

Observing sacred and profane in animist worldview

Rodewald, Michael Kenneth, January 1900 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D. in Missiology)--Concordia Theological Seminary, Fort Wayne, 2007. / Abstract and vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 274-284) and index.
8

Observing sacred and profane in animist worldview

Rodewald, Michael Kenneth, January 1900 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D. in Missiology)--Concordia Theological Seminary, Fort Wayne, 2007. / Abstract and vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 274-284) and index.
9

Animism in two generations: an investigation of selected personality factors

Billingham, Robert Emil January 1979 (has links)
Cognitive development has, the past few years, become one of the most heavily researched areas of human development. The cognitive developmental theory of Jean Piaget (1929, 1930, 1952) has been the major focus of this research. The present study was also an investigation of one of the many aspects of Piaget's theory. The present study focused on Piaget (1929) theory of animism because animistic beliefs were believed, by Piaget, to be the most basic form of thinking. If this is indeed the case then the study of animism should lead to understanding the very basis for an individual's conception of the world. Several personality factors, both those that are inseparatable from the individual (sex, race, and locus of control), those which are primarily in the environment (parents education and family ideology), and those which are to·a great extent under the control of the child (parent/child relationship and school achievement) were investigated in terms of animism. Two hundred sixty five subjects (96 children, 95 mothers, and 74 fathers) from a public school system in northern Alabama volunteered to take part in the study. Each subject completed an .Animism Test and the parents filled out a family ideology questionnaire while the children filled out a locus of control test. The results of the study supported Piaget's (1929) theory of animistic development. However, the results also indicated that language, both the child's and adult model's, may play a significant role in the animistic development of children. / Ph. D.
10

Animism in Whitman : "Multitudes" of interpretation? /

Woodbury, Rachelle Helene, January 2006 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (M.A.)--Brigham Young University. Dept. of Humanities, Classics, and Comparative Literature, 2006. / Includes bibliographical references (p. 64-65).

Page generated in 0.0826 seconds