• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 566
  • 447
  • 230
  • 77
  • 31
  • 30
  • 28
  • 18
  • 12
  • 11
  • 9
  • 9
  • 9
  • 9
  • 9
  • Tagged with
  • 1797
  • 336
  • 308
  • 297
  • 188
  • 133
  • 133
  • 122
  • 114
  • 99
  • 96
  • 95
  • 92
  • 90
  • 82
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Drawing for communication

Pungthong, Viriya, January 2004 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Ohio State University, 2004. / Title from first page of PDF file. Document formatted into pages; contains xii, 275 p.; also includes graphics. Includes abstract and vita. Co-advisors: Vesta A.H. Daniel and Noel Mayer, Dept. of Art Education. Includes bibliographical references (p. 267-275).
2

The Influence of sketching on the visual expressions of senior high school art students

Romich, Gerald K. January 1964 (has links)
Thesis (M.Ed.)--Kutztown State College, 1964. / Source: Masters Abstracts International, Volume: 45-06, page: 2751. Typescript. Includes bibliographical references (leaf 41)
3

Disegno interno a history and investigation into drawing as a primary source in the visual arts /

Maddock, Steven H. January 1979 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison. / Typescript. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 79-81).
4

From head to hand and beyond :

Hooper, Margaret J. Unknown Date (has links)
Situated between embryonic ideas and 'finished' work are thought-image drawings, the tangible evidence of the exploratory thinking of artists. Often found in sketchbooks, these drawings are 'becoming-art', precursors to the work exhibited or performed in galleries and public places. Thought- image drawings are private drawings that reveal the uncensored hand at play, and hide in the shadows of public discourse. They are too easily ignored or forgotten, and if they are kept, they are relegated to archives where they remain, under-utilised. In this study, I explore and establish sketchbooks as dynamic, dual sites of discovery in which artists encode a plethora of ideas for themselves and for others, as long as they are saved and shown at some future time. / By developing Gilles Deleuze's and Felix Guattari's 'rhizomatic method of becoming', it is possible to employ an open-ended approach for the continual gathering and sorting of data. Similar to the eclectic action of the brain, the nomadic process of the rhizome assembles disparate knowledge during its trajectory, and pauses or plateaus from time to time to consider the state of affairs before advancing again. To this end, I critique discourse around fixed definitions of drawing as 'systems', and track an investigative path through public archives, private studios and educational institutions. I demonstrate that sketchbooks contain a collection of possibilities rather than certainties and possess the potential to reframe ways in which drawing can be thought and taught. Where the drawing methods found in sketchbooks do not necessarily conform to traditional western systems, I argue that broader investigation into the diverse styles therein may assist students to expand their visual oeuvre. The disparate ideas and methods of drawing found in artists' sketchbooks offer unique opportunities for art and design students to develop their art/design practice beyond the scope presented by work generally displayed in public exhibitions. / Thesis (PhD)--University of South Australia, 2008.
5

The doing of drawing :

Hooper, Margaret J. Unknown Date (has links)
Thesis (M Design)--University of South Australia, 1995
6

Het Noord- en Zuidnederlandse tekenboek 1600-1750 /

Bolten, J. January 1900 (has links)
Thesis--Amsterdam. / Includes summary in English. Includes bibliographical references (p. 230-259).
7

An exploration of color drawing mediums / Color drawing mediums.

McKee, Thomas W. January 1986 (has links)
The creative project dealt with an intensive study of a wide variety of color drawing media and surface grounds (papers). The artist carried out a number of sketches to determine the properties of the individual drawing materials, then executed a series of eight finished drawings. The results were analyzed and presented in written documentation. In addition, the artist also researched historical and stylistic relationships between his own and existing works.The main accomplishments of this project were the development and growth of subject matter and the experimentation with materials and technique which occured in the artist's work. Through work in the various media, the artist attained a deeper understanding of the technical application of the materials and further developed and refined his range of images and subject matter.
8

Perceptual training in drawing among students from two countries a multi-cultural study /

Richards, Allan G. Rennels, Max R. January 1987 (has links)
Thesis (Ed. D.)--Illinois State University, 1987. / Title from title page screen, viewed August 3, 2005. Dissertation Committee: Max R. Rennels (chair), Susan F. Amster, Ronald S. Halinski, Heather Hanlon, John R. McCarthy. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 65-66) and abstract. Also available in print.
9

Drawing Accuracy, Quality and Expertise

Carson, Linda Christine January 2012 (has links)
Drawing from a still-life is a complex visuomotor task. Nevertheless, experts depict three-dimensional subjects convincingly with two-dimensional images. Drawing research has previously been limited by its general dependence on qualitative assessment of drawings by human critics and on retrospective self-report of expertise by drawers. Accuracy measures cannot hope to encompass all the properties of “goodness” in a drawing but this thesis will show that they are consistent with the expertise of the drawers and with the quality ratings of human critics, they are robust enough to support analysis of ecologically valid drawing tasks from complex three-dimensional stimuli, and they are sensitive enough to study global and local properties of drawings. Drawing expertise may depend to some extent on more accurate internal models of 3D space. To explore this possibility we had adults with a range of drawing experience draw a still life. We measured the angles at intersecting edges in the drawings to calculate each person's mean percentage magnitude error across angles in the still life. This gave a continuous objective measure of drawing accuracy which correlated well with years of art experience. Participants also made perceptual judgements of still lifes, both from direct observation and from an imagined side view. A conventional mental rotation task failed to differentiate drawing expertise. However, those who drew angles more accurately were also significantly better judges of slant, i.e., the pitch of edges in the still life. Those with the most drawing experience were significantly better judges of spatial extent, i.e., which landmarks were leftmost, rightmost, nearest, farthest etc.. The ability to visualize in three dimensions the orientation and relationships of components of a still life is related to drawing accuracy and expertise. In our second study, we set out to extend our understanding of drawing accuracy and to develop measures that would support more complex research questions about both drawing and visual perception. We developed and applied novel objective geometric measures of accuracy to analyze a perspective drawing task. We measured the deformation of shapes in drawings relative to the ground truth of a reference photograph and validated these measures by showing that they discriminate appropriately between experts and novices. On all measures—orientation, proportionality, scale and position of shapes—experts outperformed novices. However, error is not uniform across the image. Participants were better at capturing the proportions and positions of objects (the “positive space”) than of the spaces between those objects (the “negative space”) and worse at orienting those objects than shapes in the negative space, but scale error did not differ significantly between positive and negative space. We have demonstrated that objective geometric measures of drawing accuracy are consistent with expertise and that they can be applied to new levels of analysis, not merely to support the conventional wisdom of art educators but to develop new, evidence-based means of training this fundamental skill. Most or all prior research into drawing was based on human ratings of drawing quality, but we cannot take for granted that the “goodness” of a drawing is related to its accuracy. In order to determine whether our objective measures of accuracy are consistent with drawing quality, we invited more than one hundred participants to grade the quality of all of the drawings we had collected and measured. We showed participants photographs of the still lifes on which the drawings were based and asked them to grade the quality of each drawing on a scale from 1 (“Poor”) to 10 (“Excellent”). People's quality ratings were consistent with one another. People without drawing experience rated drawings slightly more highly than the drawing experts did, but the ratings of both groups correlated well. As we predicted, the more drawing experience the artist had, the more highly rated the drawing was, and the more accurate the drawing was, the more highly rated it was. Furthermore, scaling error (but not proportionality, orientation or position) also predicted drawing quality. In perspective drawing, accuracy—as measured by angle error or polygon error—is related to drawing quality. If drawing practice strengthens an artist's model of 3D space, we would expect the three-dimensionality of drawings to be disrupted by damage to the dorsal stream or the connection between the dorsal and ventral streams. A former illustrator and animator, DM, who had suffered a right hemisphere stroke and presented with spatial neglect, performed modified versions of the angle judgement, spatial judgement and indirect drawing tasks of our second study. Despite his previous experience, he showed weaknesses in his mental model of 3D space, weaknesses that were not evident in his drawings before the stroke. Taken together, the thesis has developed and validated two objective measures of drawing accuracy that both capture expert/novice differences well and provide superior measures when contrasted with self-reported expertise. The performance of a single patient with neglect highlights the potential involvement of the dorsal stream in drawing. The novel quantitative measures developed here allow for testable hypotheses concerning the cognitive and neural mechanisms that support the complex skill of drawing to be objectively measured.
10

Drawing Accuracy, Quality and Expertise

Carson, Linda Christine January 2012 (has links)
Drawing from a still-life is a complex visuomotor task. Nevertheless, experts depict three-dimensional subjects convincingly with two-dimensional images. Drawing research has previously been limited by its general dependence on qualitative assessment of drawings by human critics and on retrospective self-report of expertise by drawers. Accuracy measures cannot hope to encompass all the properties of “goodness” in a drawing but this thesis will show that they are consistent with the expertise of the drawers and with the quality ratings of human critics, they are robust enough to support analysis of ecologically valid drawing tasks from complex three-dimensional stimuli, and they are sensitive enough to study global and local properties of drawings. Drawing expertise may depend to some extent on more accurate internal models of 3D space. To explore this possibility we had adults with a range of drawing experience draw a still life. We measured the angles at intersecting edges in the drawings to calculate each person's mean percentage magnitude error across angles in the still life. This gave a continuous objective measure of drawing accuracy which correlated well with years of art experience. Participants also made perceptual judgements of still lifes, both from direct observation and from an imagined side view. A conventional mental rotation task failed to differentiate drawing expertise. However, those who drew angles more accurately were also significantly better judges of slant, i.e., the pitch of edges in the still life. Those with the most drawing experience were significantly better judges of spatial extent, i.e., which landmarks were leftmost, rightmost, nearest, farthest etc.. The ability to visualize in three dimensions the orientation and relationships of components of a still life is related to drawing accuracy and expertise. In our second study, we set out to extend our understanding of drawing accuracy and to develop measures that would support more complex research questions about both drawing and visual perception. We developed and applied novel objective geometric measures of accuracy to analyze a perspective drawing task. We measured the deformation of shapes in drawings relative to the ground truth of a reference photograph and validated these measures by showing that they discriminate appropriately between experts and novices. On all measures—orientation, proportionality, scale and position of shapes—experts outperformed novices. However, error is not uniform across the image. Participants were better at capturing the proportions and positions of objects (the “positive space”) than of the spaces between those objects (the “negative space”) and worse at orienting those objects than shapes in the negative space, but scale error did not differ significantly between positive and negative space. We have demonstrated that objective geometric measures of drawing accuracy are consistent with expertise and that they can be applied to new levels of analysis, not merely to support the conventional wisdom of art educators but to develop new, evidence-based means of training this fundamental skill. Most or all prior research into drawing was based on human ratings of drawing quality, but we cannot take for granted that the “goodness” of a drawing is related to its accuracy. In order to determine whether our objective measures of accuracy are consistent with drawing quality, we invited more than one hundred participants to grade the quality of all of the drawings we had collected and measured. We showed participants photographs of the still lifes on which the drawings were based and asked them to grade the quality of each drawing on a scale from 1 (“Poor”) to 10 (“Excellent”). People's quality ratings were consistent with one another. People without drawing experience rated drawings slightly more highly than the drawing experts did, but the ratings of both groups correlated well. As we predicted, the more drawing experience the artist had, the more highly rated the drawing was, and the more accurate the drawing was, the more highly rated it was. Furthermore, scaling error (but not proportionality, orientation or position) also predicted drawing quality. In perspective drawing, accuracy—as measured by angle error or polygon error—is related to drawing quality. If drawing practice strengthens an artist's model of 3D space, we would expect the three-dimensionality of drawings to be disrupted by damage to the dorsal stream or the connection between the dorsal and ventral streams. A former illustrator and animator, DM, who had suffered a right hemisphere stroke and presented with spatial neglect, performed modified versions of the angle judgement, spatial judgement and indirect drawing tasks of our second study. Despite his previous experience, he showed weaknesses in his mental model of 3D space, weaknesses that were not evident in his drawings before the stroke. Taken together, the thesis has developed and validated two objective measures of drawing accuracy that both capture expert/novice differences well and provide superior measures when contrasted with self-reported expertise. The performance of a single patient with neglect highlights the potential involvement of the dorsal stream in drawing. The novel quantitative measures developed here allow for testable hypotheses concerning the cognitive and neural mechanisms that support the complex skill of drawing to be objectively measured.

Page generated in 0.0649 seconds