71 |
Development of beliefs about chance and luckCornelius, Chelsea Ann 20 February 2012 (has links)
Children ages 5 and 8 dropped a marble into a box and made predictions about which of two doors the marble would exit. Participants provided explanations and certainty ratings for each of their predictions. A lucky charm was used in a second round of the game, in which half of participants experienced an increase in success and half did not. Results indicated that older children were more cognizant of the chance nature of the game, however both age groups exhibited misconceptions about the predictability of chance outcomes. When asked to explain their overall success in Round 2, only 8 year-olds who experienced an increase in success and a perfect success rate reliably endorsed the lucky charm. Results are discussed with reference to literature on children’s and adults’ understanding of chance. We also discuss developmental patterns in the use of luck as an explanatory tool. / text
|
72 |
A critical examination of actuarial offender-based prediction assessments: guidance for the next generation of assessmentsConnolly, Michele Moczygemba 28 August 2008 (has links)
Not available / text
|
73 |
Teachers' perception of use of student performance information: technology acceptance modelYang, Sung-kwan 28 August 2008 (has links)
Not available / text
|
74 |
Proposed llfe prediction model for an automotive wheelMcGrath, PJ 01 January 2004 (has links)
Summary
Historicully, stress analysis used for component design
assames "ideal" materials, i.e. with isotropic,
homogeneous and uniform metallurgical properties.
We know that this is untrue, no matter how good
the design. As design cannot provide fo, defect-free
materials or components, ok appropriute defect tolerance
should be the aim of the designer. Hence the
concept of fuil-safe components has been introduced
fo, safety-critical purts. Automotive wheels, howeveF,
ure not considered fail-safe and fatigue lW
prediction techniques fo, these components need to
be improved in an endeavour to provide light-weight,
attractive, but still safe und durable wheels. This
applied approuch, where the proposed lW prediction
model employs relutionships given by Gerber und a
proposed lW prediction model derived from combining
aspects of lW prediction models according to
Collins und Juvinall & Marshek. The results show
good coruelation with that of uctual wheel
fatigue
data.
|
75 |
The relationships to academic achievement of responses to a personality questionnaireClifford, Richard Cranston, 1927- January 1955 (has links)
No description available.
|
76 |
Predictive inferenceMiller, J. Glenn (James) 05 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
77 |
Selection rules for building transfer function modelsGutierrez-Carmona, Rafael Silviano 08 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
78 |
The method to predict a large earthquake in an aftershock sequenceCreamer, Frederic Harold 08 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
79 |
Casual judgements from contingency information : competition between multiple causes of a single outcomeDarredeau, Christine. January 2007 (has links)
In causal reasoning the presence of a strong predictor of an outcome interferes with causal judgments of a moderate co-occurring predictor. Causal competition effects have generally been demonstrated with a strong competing predictor that is followed by an outcome with a higher probability than a moderate target predictor, and that also signals as many or more of the total outcome occurrences than the moderate target predictor. Confounding these two distinct aspects of predictiveness has constrained the ability to examine their respective importance for the relative validity of predictors in causal competition. By examining the effects of one and two strong competing causes on judgments of a moderate cause, varying the proportion of total outcomes that the competing predictors are paired with while holding overall outcome frequency constant, this series of experiments begins to disentangle these aspects of predictiveness. It demonstrates competition effects with a strong predictor that predicts fewer outcomes than the moderate target predictor. In addition, causal competition was examined between positive predictors (those signaling the occurrence of the outcome), between negative predictors (those signaling the absence of the outcome) and between predictors of opposite polarity (positive and negative). Causal candidates of opposite polarity were found to enhance rather than reduce causal judgments of moderate positive and negative predictors, posing a challenge for some of the most influential theories of causal learning that explain competition effects as the discounting of the moderate predictor or a failure to learn its association with the outcome. Rather, these results are consistent with a contrast mechanism whereby causal judgments of moderate predictors are not necessarily reduced toward zero in the presence of stronger predictors, but are adjusted along the causal judgment scale in opposite direction from the strong predictors. When the competing predictors are of the same polarity causal judgments of moderate predictors appear to be reduced, but when they are of opposite polarity judgments are enhanced. The implications for various associative and statistical models of causal learning are discussed.
|
80 |
Multivariate statistical prediction and ultrasound blood flow dataPercy, David Frank January 1989 (has links)
No description available.
|
Page generated in 0.0696 seconds