• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 7
  • 5
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 18
  • 18
  • 11
  • 10
  • 6
  • 5
  • 5
  • 5
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • 3
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Aspects of spatial thinking in geography textbook questions

Jo, Injeong 15 May 2009 (has links)
This study examined questions embedded in four high school world geography textbooks to evaluate the degree to which the three components of spatial thinking were incorporated: concepts of space, tools of representation, and processes of reasoning. A three-dimensional taxonomy of spatial thinking to assess the questions was developed and validated via a survey of a group of spatial thinking experts. The spatiality of the concepts featured in 3,010 questions sampled from the textbooks was analyzed. The degree to which spatial representations and stimuli for reasoning were presented was also measured. Every question was compared against the taxonomy and coded. Inter-coder reliability was measured on about one percent of the sample questions. The results indicated that most questions that required knowledge about spatial concepts could be answered by knowing only simple concepts, such as location and place-specific identity, rather than complex concepts that require the identification of spatial patterns and associations. Not many questions asked students to incorporate spatial representations to answer the questions. Few questions did require creating a new representation. Students were asked to recall memorized geographic knowledge and terms rather than to infer, hypothesize, and generalize. Little difference was found among the four textbooks in that they rarely integrated the three components of spatial thinking into the questions. The research found that page-margin questions involved aspects of spatial thinking more than section- and chapter-assessment questions. Relatively simple concepts and lower level cognitive processes, however, were required in most questions that integrated the three components. The development of questions to help students practice complex processes of spatial thinking is necessary. The taxonomy developed in this research can be used as a guide to design curricular, instructional materials, and questions that incorporate aspects of spatial thinking.
2

Aspects of spatial thinking in geography textbook questions

Jo, Injeong 15 May 2009 (has links)
This study examined questions embedded in four high school world geography textbooks to evaluate the degree to which the three components of spatial thinking were incorporated: concepts of space, tools of representation, and processes of reasoning. A three-dimensional taxonomy of spatial thinking to assess the questions was developed and validated via a survey of a group of spatial thinking experts. The spatiality of the concepts featured in 3,010 questions sampled from the textbooks was analyzed. The degree to which spatial representations and stimuli for reasoning were presented was also measured. Every question was compared against the taxonomy and coded. Inter-coder reliability was measured on about one percent of the sample questions. The results indicated that most questions that required knowledge about spatial concepts could be answered by knowing only simple concepts, such as location and place-specific identity, rather than complex concepts that require the identification of spatial patterns and associations. Not many questions asked students to incorporate spatial representations to answer the questions. Few questions did require creating a new representation. Students were asked to recall memorized geographic knowledge and terms rather than to infer, hypothesize, and generalize. Little difference was found among the four textbooks in that they rarely integrated the three components of spatial thinking into the questions. The research found that page-margin questions involved aspects of spatial thinking more than section- and chapter-assessment questions. Relatively simple concepts and lower level cognitive processes, however, were required in most questions that integrated the three components. The development of questions to help students practice complex processes of spatial thinking is necessary. The taxonomy developed in this research can be used as a guide to design curricular, instructional materials, and questions that incorporate aspects of spatial thinking.
3

Využití programu Sketchup k rozvoji prostorového myšlení u~žáků základních škol. / Using Sketchup to develop spatial inteligency of lower secondary school pupils.

MACOURKOVÁ, Renata January 2016 (has links)
This thesis is aimed on the improvement of the spatial thinking with the use of a computer programme SketchUp. In the theoretical part I focus on a brief characteristic of the programme SketchUp, definitions of the spatial thinking, its development, use, appearance in RVP ZV and in the textbooks for primary school and its improvement. Main parts of the thesis are several exercises, which I created, and their purpose is to improve the spatial thinking. All of the exercises are realized in the programme SketchUp. The thesis contains methodological comments, aims and solutions of the particular exercises.
4

Effects of a GIS Course on Three Components of Spatial Literacy

Kim, Minsung 2011 December 1900 (has links)
This research investigated whether completing an introductory GIS course affects college students' spatial literacy as defined by spatial habits of mind, spatial concepts and thinking skills, and critical spatial thinking. This study employed three tests (spatial habits of mind inventory, spatial concepts and skills test, critical spatial thinking oral test) to measure students' performance on these three elements. Furthermore, this research investigated the relationship among the components. Pre- and post-tests were conducted at the beginning and the end of the 2010 fall semester, and Texas A&M undergraduate students participated in the research. The following four research questions were examined. The first research question investigated whether GIS learning improves spatial habits of mind (n = 168). Five sub-dimensions of spatial habits of mind (pattern recognition, spatial description, visualization, spatial concept use, and spatial tool use) were identified. Overall, GIS students' spatial habits of mind were enhanced. However, variations existed when considering students' performance by dimension. The second research question explored whether GIS learning affects students' understanding and use of spatial concepts and thinking skills (n = 171). This research found that the GIS course was beneficial in improving students' spatial cognition. Students increased their understanding of key spatial concepts and applied conceptual understanding into wider contexts with advanced spatial thinking skills. The third research question examined the effects of a GIS course through interviews on the three sub-dimensions of critical spatial thinking: data reliability, spatial reasoning, and problem-solving validity (n = 32). The quantitative analyses indicated that participants developed their ability regarding these three sub-dimensions of critical spatial thinking. In particular, their ability to assess data reliability and problem-solving validity improved, an effect not likely to be enhanced by other coursework. Findings from qualitative thematic analysis confirmed these quantitative outcomes. The final research question probed the relationships among the three components of spatial literacy. Pearson?s correlation coefficient, a 3D space (termed "score space" in this study), a test for independence, and an exploratory factor analysis suggested that the three components are positively correlated. However, more research is necessary to confirm the results reported in this study.
5

Neuro-imaging Support for the Use of Audio to Represent Geospatial Location in Cartographic Design

Brittell, Megen 30 April 2019 (has links)
Audio has the capacity to display geospatial data. As auditory display design grapples with the challenge of aligning the spatial dimensions of the data with the dimensions of the display, this dissertation investigates the role of time in auditory geographic maps. Three auditory map types translate geospatial data into collections of musical notes, and arrangement of those notes in time vary across three map types: sequential, augmented-sequential, and concurrent. Behavioral and neuroimaging methods assess the auditory symbology. A behavioral task establishes geographic context, and neuroimaging provides a quantitative measure of brain responses to the behavioral task under recall and active listening response conditions. In both behavioral and neuroimaging data, two paired contrasts measure differences between the sequential and augmented-sequential map types, and between the augmented- sequential and concurrent map types. Behavioral data reveal differences in both response time and accuracy. Response times for the augmented-sequential map type are substantially longer in both contrasts under the active response condition. Accuracy is lower for concurrent maps than for augmented-sequential maps; response condition influences direction of differences in accuracy between the sequential and augmented-sequential map types. Neuroimaging data from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) show significant differences in blood-oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) response during map listening. The BOLD response is significantly stronger in the left auditory cortex and planum temporale for the concurrent map type in contrast to the augmented- sequential map type. And the response in the right auditory cortex and bilaterally in the visual cortex is significantly stronger for augmented-sequential maps in contrast to sequential maps. Results from this research provide empirical evidence to inform choices in the design of auditory cartographic displays, enriching the diversity of geographic map artifacts. Four supplemental files and two data sets are available online. Three audio files demonstrate the three map types: sequential (Supplementary Files, Audio 1), augmented- sequential (Supplementary Files, Audio 2), and concurrent (Supplementary Files, Audio 3). Associated data are available through OpenNeuro (https://openneuro.org/ datasets/ds001415).
6

College Students‘ GIS Spatial Concept Knowledge Assessed by Concept Maps

Oda, Katsuhiko 2011 May 1900 (has links)
The development of spatial thinking proficiency has been increasingly demanded in Geographic Information System (GIS) education. Despite this educational trend, there is little empirical research on college students' spatial concept knowledge, which critically affects the quality of spatial thinking. This study addressed the following three research questions: 1) What differences exist between students' understandings of spatial concepts at the beginning, middle, and end of an introductory-level GIS course?, 2) What spatial misconceptions students may possess while taking an introductory-level GIS course?, and 3) Which spatial concepts are easy or hard for undergraduate students to understand? The researcher asked twelve participants who were taking an introductory-level GIS course to create concept maps about space and revised their concept maps in three experiment sessions. For the first question, the researcher scored the sixty obtained concept maps and statistically analyzed those scores to examine if there is any significant difference among the scores of the three experiment sessions. For the second question, the researcher examined participants' misconceptions by analyzing the incorrect statements of distortion, map projection, and scale. For the third question, the researcher statistically analyzed concept-based scores to examine if there is any significant difference among the scores of three different complexity levels. A main finding for the first question was that there was a significant difference among the scores of the concept maps created in the first session and the scores of the concept maps revised in the second and third sessions. This implied that participants could successfully revise their own original concept maps in the middle of a semester. The result of the study of the second question indicated that a half of participants misunderstood the concepts of map projections and scale. This result suggested that some undergraduate students may have difficulty shifting from scientifically inappropriate spatial concept knowledge to appropriate knowledge. Analysis of the third question resulted that the concept-based scores of simple spatial concepts are significantly higher than the scores of complicated spatial concepts. This result inferred that participants' scores decreased as the complexity of the concepts increased.
7

Det rumsliga tänkandet : Gymnasieelevers geospatiala förmåga i relation till geografiämnets styrdokument.

Kristiansson, Torbjörn January 2016 (has links)
The geography subject in Swedish upper secondary school is in a marginalized position with just two national programs having it as a mandatory subject. No mention of the skill of spatial thinking is made in the regulation documents from the Swedish National Agency for Education. To investigate the spatial thinking in the Swedish upper secondary school, I constructed a test that 140 students conducted. From the results it was possible to deduct that the students performed well in areas of spatial thinking that deals with geographical features like points, lines and polygons, but not so well when faced with tasks concerning several spatial facts, overlay, and mentally visualizing 3-D images from 2-D information. Males generally scored higher than females in the test, especially if only answers that the students felt confident of were counted. Students from the Natural Science Programme outperformed the students from the Social Science Programme, even though the former didn’t have any formal education in geography from the upper secondary school. The thesis argues that a larger focus on spatial thinking in the geography subject could benefit the development of both a stronger stance for the subject in the education system, and the abilities and knowledge tied to spatial thinking of the students, especially those that are lacking formal education in other subjects that train the spatial thinking, mostly mathematics and physics.
8

O pensamento espacial na educação infantil: uma relação entre geografia e cartografia / Spatial thinking in childrens education: the relationship between geography and cartography

Juliasz, Paula Cristiane Strina 27 April 2017 (has links)
O pensamento espacial é uma atividade cognitiva desenvolvida no cotidiano e pode ser sistematizado pelas mais diversas disciplinas escolares, principalmente pela Geografia. Os conceitos, as representações e habilidades espaciais são componentes dessa forma de pensamento. A compreensão do conceito de pensamento espacial e a investigação de como pode ser desenvolvido de forma sistematizada na escola torna-se ponto central na contemporaneidade, composta pelas mais diversas linguagens que representam o espaço. Reconhecendo tal importância, constatamos a ausência de referenciais para o seu desenvolvimento por crianças em atividades escolares, com o enfoque geográfico, na Educação Infantil. Como objetivo principal, destaca-se a proposição de referenciais teóricometodológicos para o conhecimento espacial de crianças de quatro a seis anos. Parte-se da seguinte pergunta de pesquisa: quais habilidades e conceitos espaciais podem ser abordados em atividades para o desenvolvimento do pensamento espacial de crianças de 4 a 6 anos? Para respondê-la e alcançarmos o objetivo principal, os objetivos específicos constituem pauta de trabalho para o desenvolvimento da investigação: sondar e analisar a pertinência, as possibilidades e a abordagem das noções espaciais na Educação Infantil; desenvolver situações de ensino, com base nas teorias norteadoras sobre pensamento espacial, desenho infantil e construção de conceito sob a perspectiva histórico-cultural; compreender os padrões estabelecidos nas representações gráficas realizadas pelas crianças; compreender os diálogos entre as crianças. As sequências de atividades são instrumentos da investigação, cada uma apresenta seus objetivos próprios, com base na representação, no conceito e nas habilidades do pensamento espacial. As três sequências de atividades têm como conceito central a Localização. A primeira trata especificamente do espaço próximo, aquele de vivência, enquanto as outras duas mobilizam o conhecimento geográfico a partir de aspectos distantes de suas vivências imediatas. Para analisarmos a expressão do pensamento espacial, narramos os acontecimentos principais da atividade, transcrevemos as vídeo-gravações e mapeamos os registros de acordo com o nosso interesse: verificar os conceitos e habilidades espaciais. Compreendemos que o desenho consiste em uma linguagem que concretiza e reflete o raciocínio espacial, envolvendo importantes princípios da cartografia e da geografia. A concepção assumida é a da infância enquanto categoria social e a criança enquanto sujeito de direito ao conhecimento e, neste caso, espacial. A criança é pensada nesta pesquisa como ser capaz de aprender, refletir, criar, trocar, dialogar e ensinar sobre o espaço. Ao longo da análise dos dados de pesquisa, concluímos que as palavras são elementos fundamentais que concretizam o modo de pensar, no caso, a habilidade do pensamento espacial, e o desenho é parte da iniciação cartográfica. Afirmamos nesta tese a relação direta da Geografia no desenvolvimento do pensamento espacial, tendo em vista a própria natureza desta ciência, e também da Cartografia enquanto linguagem que materializa essa forma de pensar. Defendese, assim,o conhecimento geográfico na Educação Infantil, por meio do pensamento espacial, pois as crianças podem ampliar seus conhecimentos espaciais. / Spatial thinking, comprised of concepts, representations and spatial abilities, is a cognitive activity developed in everyday living, and can be systematized through different school disciplines, mainly Geography. The comprehension of this concept and the investigation of how it can be developed and systematized in schools are critical points, involving different languages that represent the space. Considering this, we noticed the absence of references in geography-focused school activities for the development of spatial thinking. Our main objective is to propose theoretical and methodological references for the spatial knowledge of children aged between 4 and 6 years old. The research is based on the following question: Which spatial abilities and concepts can be addressed in activities aimed at developing spatial thinking in children aged 4 to 6 years old? To answer this question and achieve the main objective, the specific objectives were: a) to investigate and analyze the pertinence, possibilities and approaches regarding the spatial notions in childrens education; b) develop teaching situations based on guiding theories about spatial thinking, childrens drawing and the concept construction under a historical and cultural perspective; c) understand the patterns in childrens graphic representations; and d) analyze the childrens dialogues. Three series of activities with distinct objectives were proposed and analyzed as investigation instruments, based on the spatial thinking representation, concept, and abilities. All the activities addressed the central concept of Location. The first activity series deals specifically with the space nearby, the existence space, while the two other series are related to the geographic knowledge regarding distant aspects. In order to analyze spatial thinking expression, we narrated the main outcomes of the activities, transcribed the video recordings, and mapped the resulting data according to our interests; i.e., verify the spatial concepts and abilities. Drawing consists in a language that concretizes and reflects spatial reasoning, involving important principles of Cartography and Geography. The present research adopted the conception of childhood as a social category, and the child is the subject of the right to acquire knowledge, in this case, spatial knowledge. In this research, children are considered capable of learning, reflecting, creating, exchanging experiences, dialoguing, and teaching about the space. The analysis of the research data allowed us to conclude that drawing is part of the cartographic initiation, and words are fundamental elements that concretize the way of thinking, in this case, spatial thinking ability. In this thesis, we reaffirm the direct relationship between Geography and the development of spatial thinking, considering the very nature of this Science, and Cartography as the language used to materialize this way of thinking. The present thesis defends the development of spatial thinking in childrens education as a way to broaden the spatial knowledge and the construction of spatial concepts by the children.
9

Educação geográfica, cartografia escolar e pensamento espacial no segundo segmento do Ensino Fundamental / Geography education, school cartography and spatial thinking in Brazilian Junior High School

Duarte, Ronaldo Goulart 28 July 2016 (has links)
O autor deste trabalho parte pressupõe que a articulação entre a Educação Geográfica e a Cartografia Escolar possui grande relevância para o desenvolvimento do pensamento espacial dos estudantes da escola básica. Essa dimensão da inteligência é essencial para a cidadania consciente e para o exercício das mais diversas atividades profissionais e cotidianas. Considerando esse cenário, o objetivo central desta pesquisa é avaliar qual é o nível de contribuição da Educação Geográfica e da Cartografia Escolar no segmento do 6º ao 9º anos do ensino fundamental para que os alunos sejam capazes de ampliar sua capacidade de pensar espacialmente em situações que envolvem representações espaciais, sobretudo as cartográficas, aplicadas a contextos geográficos. Para isso, recorremos ao campo de pesquisas do pensamento espacial (spatial thinking), desenvolvido principalmente nos Estados Unidos no decorrer das últimas décadas. Dessa forma, adotamos como definição do pensamento espacial a que foi apresentada no relatório do National Research Council (2006) e que tornou-se a referência nessa seara de investigação: Pensamento espacial um tipo de pensamento é baseado na amálgama de três elementos: conceitos espaciais, instrumentos de representação e processos de raciocínio (NRC, 2006, ix). Nossa metodologia envolveu dois caminhos distintos, mas articulados. Realizamos a análise das três coleções de livros didáticos de Geografia mais adotadas no segmento de 6º ao 9º anos do ensino fundamental brasileiro, com a finalidade de avaliar em que medida os exercícios e atividades presentes nessas obras contribuem para o desenvolvimento do pensamento espacial discente. A título de parâmetro comparativo, fizemos o mesmo com uma coleção de didáticos franceses destinados à mesma faixa de escolaridade. Para realizar essa avaliação das atividades utilizamos a Taxonomia do Pensamento Espacial de Jo e Bednarz (2009), criada exatamente para essa finalidade e replicada por Scholz et al. (2014). O levantamento identificou não apenas a proporção de questões demandantes do pensamento espacial, mas o nível dos processos de raciocínio envolvidos e o tipo de representação espacial utilizado. O segundo caminho metodológico foi a aplicação do Teste de Aptidão do Pensamento Espacial (STAT, em inglês), desenvolvido por Lee e Bednarz (2012), a 268 alunos brasileiros do 9º ano do ensino fundamental, em seis escolas diferentes. Após a tabulação dos dados realizamos uma análise comparativa entre os resultados dos alunos brasileiros e os números resultantes das aplicações do mesmo teste em dois contextos internacionais: uma escola estadunidense (LEE e BEDNARZ, 2012) e os de três escolas em Ruanda (TOMASZEWSKI et al., 2014). A análise incluiu tanto os resultados globais do teste quanto os resultados dos alunos de cada instituição em cada um dos oito grupos de modalidades do pensamento espacial considerados pelos elaboradores do STAT. Nossas conclusões finais resultaram do cotejo das informações obtidas a partir dessas duas linhas metodológicas. / The author of this thesis believes that Geography Education and School Cartography have both great importance for the development of spatial thinking among students in K-12. This dimension of intelligence is crucial for citizenship and for the practice of many professional and daily activities. Considering such a framework, the main goal of this research is to assess the contribution of Geography Education and School Cartography in Brazilian middle school to enhance students capacity to think spatially in situations that encompass spatial representations, specially cartographic ones, in geographical contexts. To answer this question we decided to betake the field research known as spatial thinking, developed mostly in the U.S. during the last two decades. As a consequence we adopted the definition of spatial thinking that was presented in the report of the National Research Council (2006), which has become the main reference in the field: Spatial thinkingone form of thinking is based on a constructive amalgam of three elements: concepts of space, tools of representation, and processes of reasoning (NRC, 2006, ix). Our methodology was based on two interconnected paths. We analyzed the three most adopted Geography textbooks collections for the four years of Brazilian middle school (6th to 9th grades) with the purpose of assessing in which extent the exercises in those books are capable of fostering students spatial thinking. In order to stablish a comparison with Brazilian textbooks we also assessed a French textbook collection destined to the same grade levels. To perform such an assessment, we used the Taxonomy of Spatial Thinking (Jo and Bednarz, 2009) designed for this particular purpose and replied by Scholz et al. (2014). The study identified not only the ratio of questions demanding spatial thinking but also the processes of reasoning involved. The second methodological path was based on the use of the Spatial Thinking Ability Test (STAT), designed by Lee and Bednarz (2012). We have administered the STAT to 268 students of six different Brazilian schools, all of them in the end of the 9th grade. After organizing all the data, we compared the Brazilian results with those of two international studies where the same test was administered: one school in the United States (LEE and BEDNARZ, 2012) and three schools in Ruanda (TOMASZEWSKI et al., 2014). Our analysis included both the global results of students overall performance in the test and their accomplishments regarding the eight modes of thinking spatially defined by the authors of the STAT. Our final conclusions were the results of the comparison between the outcomes obtained from our two methodological paths.
10

The Development of Early Spatial Thinking

Abad, Carla 20 March 2018 (has links)
The different spatial experiences in the lives of young boys and girls may partly explain sex differences in spatial skills (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1995; Nazareth et al., 2013; Newcombe, Bandura & Taylor, 1983). While several studies have examined the influence of spatial activities on the development of spatial skills (e.g., Nazareth et al., 2013) there currently exists no widely used comprehensive measure to assess children’s concurrent participation in spatial activities and engagement with spatial toys. Study 1 of the current dissertation filled this gap in the field of spatial research through the creation of the Spatial Activity Questionnaire, a comprehensive survey designed to assess children’s involvement in spatial activities and engagement with spatial toys of diverse gender-typed content. The toys and activities 295 children were reported to have access to and engage with were explored to assess patterns of play with spatial and gender-stereotyped toys and activities. A sample of 76 children between 4 and 6 years of age and their primary caregivers participated in studies 2, 3, and 4 to explore the toys and activities young children have access to and play with (study 2), the link between play and mental rotation (study 3), and the relation between play, gender stereotypes, and mental rotation skills (study 4). Findings reveal great variability in the toys and activities children have access to and play with, with sex difference suggesting girls play with low-spatial and stereotypically feminine toys and activities more than boys while boys play with highly-spatial and stereotypically masculine toys and activities more than girls. Adding to the exiting literature suggesting the inconsistency of sex differences in early mental rotation skills, our results suggest no sex differences in children’s mental rotation ability. Furthermore, no relations were discovered between children’s play, gender stereotypes, and mental rotation ability. These findings point to the need to further explore the influence of play on when and how sex differences in mental rotation ability develop in order to promote fun and easy ways to support spatial learning in young boys and girls.

Page generated in 0.1015 seconds