Spelling suggestions: "subject:"aggressiveness inn children."" "subject:"aggressiveness iin children.""
21 |
Characteristics of relationally aggressive fifth grade girlsFleetman, Kathy S. January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (M. Ed.)--Lancaster Bible College, 2005. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 59-66).
|
22 |
Empathy : a proposed moderator to the relationship between Machiavellianism and social aggression in Hispanic and non-Hispanic childrenReeves Washer, Shanna Raelene, 1979- 31 August 2012 (has links)
Social aggression is defined as a set of behaviors directed towards damaging another individual’s self-esteem or social status and may include direct forms such as verbal rejection, negative facial expressions or gestures, or more indirect means such as rumor spreading or social exclusion (Galen & Underwood, 1997). Previous research demonstrates that social aggression and self-reported empathy have a negative linear relationship, and that social aggression is positively correlated with the ability to effectively navigate varied social contexts (a subset of Machiavellianism, or a manipulative beliefs and strategies towards others; Wilson, Near, & Miller, 1996). Despite this evidence, few researchers have investigated the relationships among all three of these variables (empathy, social aggression, and social navigation) (Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & Kaukiainen, 2000; Kaukiainen et al., 1999). This study examines whether social aggression is correlated with Machiavellianism, or a manipulative orientation to others, and if this relationship is contingent upon the child’s self- reported level of empathy. It is hypothesized that children’s levels of Machiavellianism are related to the use of social aggression, but the strength of this relationship will vary according to level of empathy. Therefore, empathy is proposed to serve as a moderator of the relationship between Machiavellianism and social aggression. The investigator also conducted additional exploratory analyses using a portion of the sample identified by school records as Hispanic. The same relationships are explored with this portion of the sample to determine whether this population is unique with respect to these relationships. Using this portion of the sample, intercorrelations among the variables are reported as well. A sample of 280, nine- to 13-year-old students from a school district outside a metropolitan area participated. Each one completed instruments designed to measure social aggression, empathy, Machiavellianism, and acculturation. The results indicated that there was no linear relationship between Machiavellianism and social aggression, and further, there is no moderating effect of empathy. An exploratory analysis with Hispanic participants (n = 217) suggests the relationship between Machiavellianism and social aggression, and non-significant role of empathy as a moderator, do not differ for Hispanic and non-Hispanic participants. Interestingly, acculturation is positively correlated with social aggression and negatively correlated with empathy, suggesting that as children become more oriented to US culture, they are more likely to engage in social aggression and report lower overall levels of empathy. These findings contribute uniquely to the literature, and further, provide new information on these constructs using a Hispanic sample. Implications for intervention and future research are discussed. / text
|
23 |
Children's aggressive and prosocial behaviours: exploring age, sex-related differences, and the role ofsocial cognitive functioningLee, Ka-yee, Cavy., 李嘉怡. January 2010 (has links)
Children’s social behaviours have significant implications to their
adjustments. Aggressive children display emotional, behavioural, and social
problems. On the other hand, prosocial children are better adjusted (for review,
see Card, Stucky, Sawalani, & Little, 2008). Adopting the social cognitive
approach, the present study aimed to investigate the relationships between
several social-cognitive capacities related to empathy (i.e., perspective-taking,
affect sharing, and emotion regulation) and social behaviours (physical
aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behaviours) in children. The
results showed that perspective-taking was the most predictive factor of both
prosocial and aggressive behaviours, whereas the capacity to share emotions
and to modulate emotion was found to have little predictive value to both
positive and negative social behaviours. In addition, the present study also
investigated the sex-related and age differences of aggressive behaviours in
children. Consistent with the findings of previous studies, this study found that
boys enacted more physical aggression than girls. However, sex-related
difference in relational aggression was not found. Besides, older children were
found to use more relational aggression but the trend of decreasing physical
aggression across development was not evident. Clinical implications of the
present findings were discussed. / published_or_final_version / Clinical Psychology / Doctoral / Doctor of Psychology
|
24 |
From physical abuse victim to aggressor : exploring the relationshipEllenbogen, Stephen. January 2008 (has links)
This dissertation represents an attempt to better understand why physically abused children are more likely to develop aggressive tendencies. A sample of adolescents receiving child protection services was administered a battery of self-report measures on maltreatment, health, and well-being; an aggression measure was administered a year later. The specific focus was to determine whether aggressive youth were more likely to (1) have suffered harsher physical abuse, (2) generate violence outcome expectancies that favour recourse to aggression, and (3) report higher levels of abuse-related shame. Respectively, the second and third hypotheses were based on social information processing and shame-rage models of aggression. / Only the hypothesis of the fast study on maltreatment characteristics was supported. Victims of frequent and severe abuse were more likely to report aggression. As for the second and third study, the expectation of positive outcomes from violence and feelings of shame-rage (as indicated by high levels of guilt-free shame) were actually inversely related to aggression. / Even though these results went in the opposite direction of the hypotheses, they are not altogether in conflict with theoretical models. According to L. Rowell Huesmann's description of social information processing, a hostile worldview was considered a mediating factor in the development of aggression problems. The tendency for aggressive youth to expect negative rather than positive outcomes from violence can be interpreted as support for this view. Abuse-related shame was not related to aggression, but it did demonstrate the predicted association with anger and hostility. The suggestion therefore is that feeling shame as a result of abuse leads to pent-up rather than expressed rage. / Given these results, it is suggested that victims of harsh maltreatment be targeted for appropriate prevention and skills promotion programs. Also, future research should investigate whether the formation of problematic relationship schemas and the tendency to suppress abuse-related emotions contribute to the maintenance of aggression problems.
|
25 |
Maternal psychological control and realtional [sic] aggression during early childhood do maternal warmth and child temperament moderate the assoicaition [sic] ? /Lau, Yi Hung, January 2008 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (M.A. in human development)--Washington State University, May 2008. / Includes bibliographical references (p. 64-71).
|
26 |
Trajectories of co-occurring aggressive and depressive symptoms in children : prediction from child and family characteristics /Hogansen, Jennifer Malia. January 2004 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Oregon, 2004. / Typescript. Includes vita and abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 118-124). Also available for download via the World Wide Web; free to University of Oregon users.
|
27 |
Predicting 36 month child aggression from the family management, child, risk, and contextual variables /Caldwell, Sandra L., January 2004 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Washington, 2004. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 75-84).
|
28 |
Social-cognitive predictors of reactive and proactive aggression investigation in a diverse, urban 5th grade sample /McCarthy, Shauna K. January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--Bowling Green State University, 2005. / Document formatted into pages; contains viii, 123 p. Includes bibliographical references.
|
29 |
The association between aggressive behaviors and academic self-esteem of preschool childrenWalker, Chrische' Marie, January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--West Virginia University, 2006. / Title from document title page. Document formatted into pages; contains vii, 62 p. Vita. Includes abstract. Includes bibliographical references (p. 37-41).
|
30 |
Characteristics of relationally aggressive fifth grade girlsFleetman, Kathy S. January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (M. Ed.)--Lancaster Bible College, 2005. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 59-66).
|
Page generated in 0.0969 seconds