Spelling suggestions: "subject:"causal reasoning"" "subject:"causal seasoning""
1 |
Hypothesizing Device Mechanisms: Opening Up the Black BoxDoyle, Richard James 01 June 1988 (has links)
I describe an approach to forming hypotheses about hidden mechanism configurations within devices given external observations and a vocabulary of primitive mechanisms. An implemented causal modelling system called JACK constructs explanations for why a second piece of toast comes out lighter, why the slide in a tire gauge does not slip back inside when the gauge is removed from the tire, and how in a refrigerator a single substance can serve as a heat sink for the interior and a heat source for the exterior. I report the number of hypotheses admitted for each device example, and provide empirical results which isolate the pruning power due to different constraint sources.
|
2 |
Understanding And Promoting Children's Use Of WeightWang, Zhidan 09 May 2016 (has links)
Causal reasoning is an important part of scientific thinking, and even young children can use causes to explain what they observe and to make predictions. Weight is an interesting type of cause because it is a nonobvious property, and thus is not readily observable. The first research question of my dissertation examines when children use this property as a cause. In Study 1, 2- to 5-year-old children completed three different tasks in which they had to use weight to produce effects; an object displacement task, a balance-scale task, and a tower building task. The children’s use of weight improved with age, with 4- and 5-year-olds showing above-chance performance on all tasks. The younger children’s performance was more variable across tasks, suggesting that the complexity of the problem may influence their use of weight.
The second research question is whether children’s use of weight as a cause can be improved. To examine this question, I varied the pedagogical cues that children received on the balance scale task from Study 1. The results of Study 2, indicate that highlighting the different effects of the heavy and light objects improves 3- to 4-year-olds’ performance. However, the results of Study 3 indicate that 2-year-olds did not benefit from even multiple pedagogical cues (contrasting the different effects and providing a verbal description to highlight the weight difference). To sum up, children at age 4 and above showed a general ability to use weight in across causal reasoning tasks. Whether children’s understanding of weight could be improved depended on their age and the cues given.
|
3 |
Causal ReconstructionBorchardt, Gary C. 01 February 1993 (has links)
Causal reconstruction is the task of reading a written causal description of a physical behavior, forming an internal model of the described activity, and demonstrating comprehension through question answering. T his task is difficult because written d escriptions often do not specify exactly how r eferenced events fit together. This article (1) ch aracterizes the causal reconstruction problem, (2) presents a representation called transition space, which portrays events in terms of "transitions,'' or collections of changes expressible in everyday language, and (3) describes a program called PATHFINDER, which uses the transition space representation to perform causal reconstruction on simplified English descriptions of physical activity.
|
4 |
Combining Associational and Causal Reasoning to Solve Interpretation and Planning ProblemsSimmons, Reid G. 01 August 1988 (has links)
This report describes a paradigm for combining associational and causal reasoning to achieve efficient and robust problem-solving behavior. The Generate, Test and Debug (GTD) paradigm generates initial hypotheses using associational (heuristic) rules. The tester verifies hypotheses, supplying the debugger with causal explanations for bugs found if the test fails. The debugger uses domain-independent causal reasoning techniques to repair hypotheses, analyzing domain models and the causal explanations produced by the tester to determine how to replace faulty assumptions made by the generator. We analyze the strengths and weaknesses of associational and causal reasoning techniques, and present a theory of debugging plans and interpretations. The GTD paradigm has been implemented and tested in the domains of geologic interpretation, the blocks world, and Tower of Hanoi problems.
|
5 |
Theories of Comparative AnalysisWeld, Daniel S. 01 May 1988 (has links)
Comparative analysis is the problem of predicting how a system will react to perturbations in its parameters, and why. For example, comparative analysis could be asked to explain why the period of an oscillating spring/block system would increase if the mass of the block were larger. This thesis formalizes the task of comparative analysis and presents two solution techniques: differential qualitative (DQ) analysis and exaggeration. Both techniques solve many comparative analysis problems, providing explanations suitable for use by design systems, automated diagnosis, intelligent tutoring systems, and explanation based generalization. This thesis explains the theoretical basis for each technique, describes how they are implemented, and discusses the difference between the two. DQ analysis is sound; it never generates an incorrect answer to a comparative analysis question. Although exaggeration does occasionally produce misleading answers, it solves a larger class of problems than DQ analysis and frequently results in simpler explanations.
|
6 |
Examination of the Belief Bias Effect across Two Domains of ReasoningMartin, Nadia January 2008 (has links)
The belief bias effect – the finding that prior beliefs influence judgments of logic and evidence – has been a topic of much empirical investigation in both deductive and causal reasoning. However, to date, no research has examined the degree to which such biases are the result of common or distinct mechanisms in these two domains. By using common scales of measurement, I examine the degree to which individuals show common biases in these two domains in two experiments. Surprisingly, although the belief bias effect was observed in both paradigms, biases in one domain were unreliably associated with biases in the other domain. Experiment 2 included 6 measures of individual differences in an attempt to uncover the observation of differential biases in these domains. Dogmatism was found to be the single most predictive measure of belief bias, but only in deductive reasoning. These data are discussed in terms of dual process theories of reasoning.
|
7 |
Examination of the Belief Bias Effect across Two Domains of ReasoningMartin, Nadia January 2008 (has links)
The belief bias effect – the finding that prior beliefs influence judgments of logic and evidence – has been a topic of much empirical investigation in both deductive and causal reasoning. However, to date, no research has examined the degree to which such biases are the result of common or distinct mechanisms in these two domains. By using common scales of measurement, I examine the degree to which individuals show common biases in these two domains in two experiments. Surprisingly, although the belief bias effect was observed in both paradigms, biases in one domain were unreliably associated with biases in the other domain. Experiment 2 included 6 measures of individual differences in an attempt to uncover the observation of differential biases in these domains. Dogmatism was found to be the single most predictive measure of belief bias, but only in deductive reasoning. These data are discussed in terms of dual process theories of reasoning.
|
8 |
Identifying Student Difficulties in Causal Reasoning for College-aged Students in Introductory Physics Laboratory ClassesOwens, Lindsay 07 June 2018 (has links)
No description available.
|
9 |
The Hows and Whys of Biological Change: Causal Flexibility in Children's ReasoningPrice, Kristin L S 10 June 2008 (has links)
No description available.
|
10 |
Cost overruns in transportation infrastructure projects: Sowing the seeds for a probabilistic theory of causationLove, P.E.D., Ahiaga-Dagbui, D.D., Irani, Zahir 2016 August 1918 (has links)
Yes / Understanding the cause of cost overruns in transportation infrastructure projects has been
a topic that has received considerable attention from academics and the popular press.
Despite studies providing the essential building blocks and frameworks for cost overrun
mitigation and containment, the problem still remains a pervasive issue for
Governments worldwide. The interdependency that exists between ‘causes’ that lead to
cost overruns materialising have largely been ignored when considering the likelihood
and impact of their occurrence. The vast majority of the cost overrun literature has tended
to adopt a deterministic approach in examining the occurrence of the phenomenon; in this
paper a shift towards the adoption of pluralistic probabilistic approach to cost overrun
causation is proposed. The establishment of probabilistic theory incorporates the ability
to consider the interdependencies of causes so to provide Governments with a holistic
understanding of the uncertainties and risks that may derail the delivery and increase
the cost of transportation infrastructure projects. This will further assist in the design of
effective mitigation and containment strategies that will ensure future transportation
infrastructure projects meet their expected costs as well as the need of taxpayers. / Australian Research Council (DP160102882)
|
Page generated in 0.3364 seconds