• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 38
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 44
  • 44
  • 21
  • 12
  • 11
  • 9
  • 8
  • 7
  • 7
  • 5
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
21

READING COMPREHENSION OF PRELINGUALLY DEAF ADOLESCENTS AND ITS RELATIONSHIP TO MATERNAL USE OF MANUAL COMMUNICATION (SIGN LANGUAGE, MOTHER, CHILDREN).

KAMPFE, CHARLENE MARIE CHIPPS. January 1984 (has links)
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between hearing mothers' methods of communication with their prelingually deaf childern, and those children's reading comprehension scores, as measured by the Stanford Achievement Test, Special Edition for Hearing Impaired Students, 1973, (SAT-HI). The subjects of this study were 201 prelingually deaf students from six randomly selected residential schools for the deaf from throughout the United States. All subjects had hearing losses of 91db (ANSI) or greater, had lost their hearing before age three, used some form of manual communication as their primary method of learning and communicating, were ages 11.25 through 19.83, had been enrolled for at least the past three years in residential schools which subscribed to the total communication philosophy, had no additional handicapping conditions, had natural mothers who were able to complete the Mother's Questionnaire, came from families who used English as the primary spoken language, and had parents whose hearing was normal. Questionnaires were sent to schools and mothers to obtain information regarding the dependent variable (reading comprehension scores); the primary independent variables (methods of communication used by mothers, ages of the children when signing mothers began to use manual communication, and skill levels of mothers who used manual communication); and a number of secondary independent variables. The date were analyzed using Pearson correlation coefficients, multiple ANOVA's, multiple regression analysis and analysis of covariance. Chronological age and non-verbal IQ acted as covariates. Results indicated that, for the subjects of this study, the method of communication used by mothers of prelingually deaf children had no significant relationship with their children's reading comprehension scores. Of the subjects whose mothers used manual communication, no significant relationship was found between reading comprehension and the ages of the children when their mothers began to sign with them. Conversely, a significant relationship was found between reading comprehension scores and signing skill levels of mothers as reported by both mothers and schools. Because of questionable validity of some of the measures, difficulties in interpretation of the ANCOVA, inability to control for ethnic background, limitations in subject selection, and the small number of subjects in Group 1; the findings of this study should be considered to be speculative, at best.
22

In mid-stream a qualitative case study of a young deaf woman--becoming 'Leigh' /

Getty, Ann Darby. January 1900 (has links)
Thesis (Ed. D.)--West Virginia University, 2009. / Title from document title page. Document formatted into pages; contains vi, 137 p. Includes abstract. Includes bibliographical references (p. 117-128).
23

The central auditory processing and continuous performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the medicated and non-medicated state

Campbell, Nicole Githa. January 2003 (has links)
Thesis (D. Phil. Communication Pathology)--University of Pretoria, 2003. / Summary in English and Afrikaans. Includes bibliographical references.
24

Audiologists' perspectives on early intervention with deaf children and their parents

Caruso, Lynn. January 2003 (has links)
Thesis (M. Ed.)--York University, 2003. Graduate Programme in Education. / Typescript. Name on certificate page : P. Lynn Caruso. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 130-140). Also available on the Internet. MODE OF ACCESS via web browser by entering the following URL: http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/yorku/fullcit?pMQ82910.
25

Assesment Profiles of Auditory Processing Disorder and Language Delay: Case Studies of Four Children

Smith, Dana Marie January 2009 (has links) (PDF)
No description available.
26

Central auditory processing disorders training and knowledge of urban Black mainstream primary school teachers in Soweto /

Hlabangwane, Grace Tintswalo. January 2002 (has links)
Thesis (M.Communication Pathology)--Universiteit van Pretoria, 2002. / Summary in English and Afrikaans. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 107-118).
27

A Study of the spoken and written language of children with impaired hearing /

Waldon, Edgar Fredrick January 1963 (has links)
No description available.
28

Representational competence of young white deaf and hearing children from different socio-economic backgrounds /

Dennis, Lila Egan January 1971 (has links)
No description available.
29

Die bruikbaarheid van die senior Suid-Afrikaanse Individuele Skaal vir die evaluering van Blanke Afrikaanssprekende, hardhorende kinders

Badenhorst, Frans Hendrik 03 1900 (has links)
Microreproduction of original thesis. / Thesis (MA)--Stellenbosch University, 1986. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: see item for full text AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: sien item vir volteks.
30

Validity and item bias of the WISC-III with deaf children.

Maller, Susan Joyce. January 1994 (has links)
The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) is likely to become the most widely used test of intelligence with deaf children, based on the popularity of the previous versions of the test. Because the test was constructed for hearing children who use spoken English, the following major research questions were asked: (a) Does the WISC-III demonstrate adequate construct validity? and (b) Do specific items exhibit differential item functioning (DIF), and does the nature of the content of each item that exhibits DIF imply that the item is biased? The test was translated into sign language and administered to a total of 110 deaf children at three different sites. The deaf children ranged from ages 8 through 16 (M = 13.25, SD = 2.37), had hearing losses identified as severe or worse, were prelingually deaf, used sign language as their primary means of communication, and were not identified as having any additional handicapping conditions. The sample of deaf children was compared to a sample of 110 hearing children similar in age and Performance IQ. Construct validity was examined using a LISREL multi-sample covariance structure analysis. The covariance structures were different (χ ² (91) = 119.42, p =.024). A Rasch Model was used to detect DIF on the following subtests: Picture Completion, Information, Similarities, Arithmetic, Vocabulary, Comprehension. All of these subtests exhibited DIF, and DIF plus the differences in mean logit ability resulted in numerous items that were more difficult for deaf children on the above Verbal subtests. Item bias was judged by examining the contents of items that exhibited DIF. Items were biased due generally to translation issues and differences in the educational curricula. Thus, deaf children are at a distinct disadvantage when taking these WISC-III subtests. Practitioners are urged to consider these findings when assessing deaf children.

Page generated in 0.0551 seconds