Spelling suggestions: "subject:"emotions"" "subject:"émotions""
11 |
Kritische Darstellung der Geschichte des Affektbegriffes (Von Descartes bis zur Gegenwart) /Bernecker, Karl, January 1915 (has links)
Greifswald, Phil. Diss. v. 20. Jan. 1915, Ref. Rehmke. / Includes bibliographical references.
|
12 |
The nature of emotion .Irons, David, January 1897 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Cornell University, 1894.
|
13 |
A study of the emotions of high school football playersFort, Moreland January 1958 (has links)
Thesis (Ed.D.)--Boston University
|
14 |
Prototype categorization of emotionFehr, Beverley Anne January 1982 (has links)
Psychologists have yet to agree on a definition of emotion. Attempts at a classical definition, whereby a concept is defined by a necessary and sufficient set of criterial attributes have not met with success. The purpose of this research, therefore, was to test the feasibility of an alternative to the classical view, namely prototype theory. According to the prototype view, concepts are organized in terms of prototypes, which are the clearest cases or best examples of the category. Category members are nonequivalent and can be ordered in terms of their degree of resemblance to the prototypical cases. Boundaries between categories are therefore ill-defined.
In this research, the feasibility of conceptualizing the everyday concept of emotion as structured in terms of prototypes was tested using Rosch's approach. Rosch and her associates have recently demonstrated that many natural language categories such as fruit, furniture, and vehicle can be conceptualized as prototypically organized. Rosch has also demonstrated that many natural language categories are organized hierarchically. For example, the set fruit, apple, Granny Smith apple illustrates a hierarchy with a superordinate, middle, and subordinate level.
The first two studies examined the hierarchical structure of emotion categories. In Study One, "emotion" was taken to be the highest, or superordinate level. Subjects
were asked to list members of the category emotion. As predicted, prototypical category members like "happiness", "anger", and "sadness", were listed first and with greater frequency than less typical members like "respect", "awe", and "boredom". The purpose of Study Two was to explore the possibility of a subordinate level of the hierarchy. Subjects were asked to list types of emotion categories generated in Study One. It was discovered that unlike the Study One results where all responses were single, words, emotion categories at this level of the hierarchy are not coded monolexemically. Subjects had to "invent" subordinate categories. Consequently there was little agreement in their responses.
Internal structure refers to that general class of conceptions of categories in which categories are composed of a core meaning and in which items within the category may be considered differentially representative of the meaning of the category term. In this research, representativeness was operationally defined by means of subjects' ratings of how good each item is as an example of its category. In Study Three, prototypicality ratings were obtained for 20 emotion terms (generated in Study One) as a measure of category representativeness. As predicted, subjects found it meaningful to rate the extent to which each instance was a good example of the category emotion. Moreover, subjects agreed with one another in their responses. Representativeness of items within a category was then shown
to affect certain dependent variables important in psychological research. Study Four concerned speed of processing. Subjects were asked to verify statements of the form "An {exemplar} is a {category name}". As predicted, response times were shorter for verification of the category membership of highly prototypical than less typical exemplars. In Study Five, subjects were given the 20 target emotions and were asked to give the general category to which each belonged. As predicted, "emotion" was given as the superordinate category name more often for prototypical than nonprototypical exemplars. In Study Six, subjects generated attributes of the 20 target emotions. A family resemblance score was computed for each emotion based on the attributes each had in common with the other category members. As predicted, prototypical category members resembled the entire family to a greater degree (i.e. had a higher family resemblance score) than nonprototypical members. Overall, the results suggested how people may organize their concept of emotion. People need not be able to define "emotion" in order to use the concept in an orderly and comprehensive way. / Arts, Faculty of / Psychology, Department of / Graduate
|
15 |
A phenomenological study of emotional experienceCheng, Ming-han, Teresa. January 1977 (has links)
published_or_final_version / abstract / toc / Clinical Psychology / Master / Master of Social Sciences
|
16 |
A cross-cultural comparison of emotional experience does culture matter? /Bhaju, Jeshmin O'Leary, Virginia E., January 2006 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis(M.S.)--Auburn University, 2006. / Abstract. Vita. Includes bibliographic references.
|
17 |
A computational model of human emotion /Warner, Robert L., January 1991 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1991. / Vita. Abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 143-144). Also available via the Internet.
|
18 |
The effects of suppressing anger on cognition and behaviourLowe, Christine A. January 2008 (has links)
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Aberdeen University, 2008. / Title from web page (viewed on July 14, 2009). Includes bibliographical references.
|
19 |
Age differences in the experience of poignancy: the roles of emotion regulation and dialectical thinking. / Age differencesJanuary 2008 (has links)
Zhang, Xin. / Thesis (M.Phil.)--Chinese University of Hong Kong, 2008. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 44-49). / Abstracts in English and Chinese. / Chapter CHAPTER ONE: --- INTRODUCTION --- p.1 / What Is Poignancy? --- p.1 / Socioemotional Selectivity Theory and Poignancy --- p.3 / Influential Psychosocial Factors --- p.4 / The Present Study --- p.9 / Chapter CHAPTER TWO: --- STUDY ONE --- p.10 / Method --- p.10 / Results and Discussion --- p.15 / Chapter CHAPTER THREE: --- STUDY TWO --- p.23 / Method --- p.23 / Results and Discussion --- p.26 / Chapter CHAPTER FOUR: --- GENERAL DISCUSSION --- p.37 / Theoretical Implications for Aging and Emotion --- p.37 / Practical Implications for Psychological Well-being of Older Adults --- p.40 / Limitation and Future Directions --- p.42 / References --- p.44 / Footnote --- p.50
|
20 |
An heuristic enquiry into clients' and therapists' experiences of emotional connection in therapyLodge, Rosemary January 2010 (has links)
This was a qualitative study looking at the experience of emotional connection in therapy from both clients’ and therapists’ perspectives. The aim was to see whether and how the experience of emotional connection was the same or different for each party; and whether and how it was connected to client change. It was an heuristic enquiry and explored the experiences of five client/therapist pairs including the researcher and the researcher’s therapist. The client and therapist in each co-researcher pair were interviewed separately about a session (chosen by the client) where both had experienced an emotional connection with each other (the researcher pair had a mutual dialogue about the experience). The interviews were then analysed using heuristic processes of immersion, incubation, illumination, explication and creative synthesis. The main findings were: (1) emotional connection was experienced on two different levels: a conscious, articulated level (the ‘manifest level’) and an unarticulated, subliminal level (the ‘hidden level’); (2) emotional connection was connected to client change on both levels; (3) the main ‘work’ of therapy took place on the manifest level; (4) however, healing of the client’s deepest, or primary hurt, took place at the hidden level; (5) at the hidden level there was a good emotional match between the client and therapist. The implications of this research are that the healing mechanisms within therapy may not always be under our conscious control and that for deep healing work it may be important for there to be a good fit at an emotional level between client and therapist.
|
Page generated in 0.0799 seconds