Spelling suggestions: "subject:"interpersonal communication inn children"" "subject:"interpersonal communication iin children""
31 |
Communication and educative intervention as essentials for the attainment of responsible adulthoodBaloyi, Wilson Mavhavaza 06 1900 (has links)
This research stems from the problems that may be encountered
in an attempt to accompany the non-adult towards proper
adulthood in the absence of both communication and educative
intervention in the educative occurrence. The educator may
fail to render his educative task adequately without communieating
with the child and intervening educati vely in his
life; and the child may be deprived of his opportunity of
becoming a responsible adult.
A human child, particularly in the industrialised societies,
is confronted by various phenomena with which he often fails
to communicate normatively. This investigation is an endeavour
to reveal the essentiality of communication during
the educative intervention, that is, in guiding the child to
refrain from immoral, non-normative and unacceptable activities
and all that violates cultural adulthood according to
the norms, values and standards prevailing in that particular
community. It further aims at disclosing that communication
in the educative sense implies educative intervention,
failing which communication becomes meaningless.
Educative intervention and communication are, in truth, inseparable
during the educative occurrence and they should
supplement and enhance each other, because their separation
may imply the nullification of the educative guidance on the
part of the educator and the denial of the child's opportunity
of attaining acceptable adulthood. In order to assist
the child to gradually actualise his adulthood, the educator
who intervenes in his life should be a devoted communicator
who strives to communicate (verbally and non-verbally) his
knowledge, feelings, beliefs and attitudes to the child while
upholding his status of adulthood. It is not expected of the
true educator to communicate well about normative adulthood
verbally and simultaneously violate this through his nonverbal
communication which includes all unacceptable physical
activities which erode the dignity of adulthood.
It implies, therefore, that in his attempt to guide the child
to comply and respect the aspects, conditions and criteria of
adulthood the educator should respect and comply with them
verbally and non-verbally. A responsible person is expected
to maintain and promote adulthood through both verbal and
non-verbal forms of communication. / Educational Studies / D. Ed. (Fundamental Pedagogics)
|
32 |
Processes and patterns of dialog between deaf and hearing siblings during playVan Horn, Dennis 11 1900 (has links)
The purpose of this study was to examine the processes and patterns of communicative
interaction which preschool and elementary school-aged deaf and hearing siblings utilized to
initiate, maintain and terminate dialogs during play. Specifically, the focus was to determine if the
processes and patterns of communication differed when a deaf sibling interacted with an older
hearing sibling who has been exposed primarily to a simultaneous visual-auditory (SimVA) pattern
of communication, as compared to when a deaf sibling interacted with a younger hearing sibling
who has been exposed to both a SimVA and a sequential visual (Seq V) pattern of communication.
Video-taped playbouts were observed between each of two sibling dyads at play within a single
family: (a) an older dyad composed of a seven-year-old hearing child and her five-year-old deaf
sister, and (b) a younger dyad with the second-born deaf sister and her three-year-old hearing
brother. The video-tapes were coded to determine: the kinds of play siblings engaged in;
the use and expression of behavioral and communicative elements of attention-getting, exchange of
information, and termination processes of dialogs; who initiated and terminated dialogs; the
occurrence of turn-taking during message delivery; and the expression of patterns of
communication used by siblings during dialogs.
Only three of five possible kinds of play were actually noted, of which social play was the
most frequently observed kind of play taking place between siblings within both dyads. In the
older hearing and deaf sibling dyad, it was found that the older hearing sister predominately used
visual processes and patterns of communicative interaction when conversing with her deaf sister,
whereas the deaf sibling relied extensively on visual-auditory processes and patterns of
communication when conversing with her hearing sister. In the younger dyad, visual-auditory
patterns of communication predominated both hearing and deaf siblings' expression of processes
and patterns of communication with each other. New terminology reflecting siblings' behavioral
and communicative patterns of communication are introduced.
This study represents the first known research examining the processes and patterns of deaf
and hearing siblings' behavioral and communicative interactions of dialog. The findings are
discussed in relation to potential applications to early intervention programs for hearing families
with deaf and hearing siblings and to future research directions. Overall, the findings from this
study appear to indicate that deaf and hearing siblings communicate in ways largely influenced by
developmental maturation and the communicative environments to which each child has been
exposed during language acquisition processes. The findings are also consistent with Vygotsky's
theory of a sociocultural origin of language development. / Education, Faculty of / Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education (ECPS), Department of / Graduate
|
33 |
Compliance-Gaining Among Anglo and Mexican-American ChildrenStroupe, Hal T. (Hal Tanner) 08 1900 (has links)
This study investigates compliance-gaining rhetoric among Anglo and Mexican-American fourth graders in three schools in north Texas. The children were asked to respond to a scenario and to give a rationale for their persuasive strategies. An analysis of interviews with 52 children indicates that although the children used some similar strategies when attempting to gain compliance from an adult, there are also some significant differences between the two cultural groups.
|
34 |
The communicative performance of nonspeaking adolescents across various participant interactionsDalton, Beth Marie. January 1986 (has links)
Call number: LD2668 .T4 1986 D34 / Master of Arts / Communication Studies
|
35 |
The Effects of a Remote Control Tactile Feedback System on Conversation Skills in Children with Autism.Shively, Jane M. 12 1900 (has links)
A few studies have examined the effects of a remote control tactile device (RCT) as an unobtrusive prompting method used to promote skills such as verbal initiations and play behaviors in children with autism. To date, however, no published studies have investigated the effects of the RCT as a consequence to increase and maintain conversation skills. This study was designed to determine whether the RCT, in conjunction with common training techniques (i.e. roleplays, visual feedback, and sibling coaching), could be used as a discrete and unobtrusive feedback system to promote conversation skills in high functioning children with autism. Results found that the RCT and training packages were effective in initially increasing rates of target responses. The effects however, did not always maintain with a return to baseline. Programming "naturally" maintaining communities of reinforcement was found to be a critical factor in the maintenance of the conversational responses.
|
36 |
Pragmatics: the verbal expression of feelingsZimmerman, Ann Paula 01 January 1982 (has links)
The purpose of this investigation was to determine at which age levels, between four and eight years, children express Praise, Apology, Commiseration, Blame, Challenge, Endearment, and both a Positive and Negative State.
Subjects were thirty children, six from each age level between four and eight years, selected from an elementary and preschool within the Portland area. Sixteen picture cards and stories were designed to elicit the eight different feelings. Each subject responded to questions at the end of the story and was given two chances to express the appropriate feeling. Each response was judged as appropriate or inappropriate and scored accordingly.
|
37 |
Communication and socialization profiles in toddlers with expressive language delaySpangle-Looney, Shawn 01 January 1988 (has links)
The purpose of this study was to compare expressive communication, receptive communication, and socialization achievement in 18- to 34-month-old ELD toddlers to the same skills in normally-speaking children. The questions this study sought to answer were, how do the three skill areas in ELD toddlers compare with the same skills in normal toddlers?, will ELD subjects evidence specific profiles of deficits involving not only expressive but receptive and social skills as well? and, within the ELD subjects will two subgroups emerge, one group having poor expressive skills only, and a second group having deficits in addition to expression.
|
38 |
Understanding and being understood negotiation in English and Japanese native and nonnative child interaction /Ibaraki, Ursula H. January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (PhD)--Macquarie University, Division of Linguistics & Psychology, Department of Linguistics, 2007. / Bibliography: 269-288.
|
39 |
Early interaction : a description of conversational turntaking in an atypical child and a group of typical children during bookreadingReid, Linda A. M. January 1987 (has links)
This study explores similarities and differences in turntaking structures in the discourse of a group of typical children and one atypical child. Nineteen normally developing pre-school children and one atypical child were videotaped reading books with their parents. Each of the nineteen parent/child dyads were videotaped at the child's pre-school, and the atypical child (Ben) was videotaped at school both with a trained educator and with his mother. Analyses of the resulting videotapes yielded categorical data on types and structures of turntaking.
The utterances of the typical children appeared most often in the category of response. This finding also applied to Ben when he was interacting with his teacher, although when Ben was interacting with his mother the majority of his utterances appeared in the category of imitation. Parents of the typical children used primarily responses, mands and turnabouts. The greatest difference between Ben's mother and the other parents is found in the categories of response and mand which were lower in the case of Ben's mother. It appears that conversational turntaking in a language delayed child is different from the pattern of conversational turntaking in a group of typical children. If indeed the This study explores similarities and differences in turntaking structures in the discourse of a group of typical children and one atypical child. Nineteen normally developing pre-school children and one atypical child were videotaped reading books with their parents. Each of the nineteen parent/child dyads were videotaped at the child's pre-school, and the atypical child (Ben) was videotaped at school both with a trained educator and with his mother. Analyses of the resulting videotapes yielded categorical data on types and structures of turntaking.
The utterances of the typical children appeared most often in the category of response. This finding also applied to Ben when he was interacting with his teacher, although when Ben was interacting with his mother the majority of his utterances appeared in the category of imitation. Parents of the typical children used primarily responses, mands and turnabouts. The greatest difference between Ben's mother and the other parents is found in the categories of response and mand which were lower in the case of Ben's mother. It appears that conversational turntaking in a language delayed child is different from the pattern of conversational turntaking in a group of typical children. If indeed the difficulty lies with interaction, or turntaking skills, this may have significant implications for approaches to remediation used with children who are identified as autistic or severely learning disabled. / Education, Faculty of / Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education (ECPS), Department of / Graduate
|
40 |
Language brokering: A parent's perspectiveSloan, Lucy 01 January 2003 (has links)
Language brokering is known as the translating transaction between two individuals. This study seeks to investigate the language brokering which occurs between Latino parents and their children. In particular, it seeks to examine Latino parents' comfort level in different brokering settings/places and with different individuals.
|
Page generated in 0.2368 seconds