31 |
Presa di decisione in situazioni rischiose: effetto della rabbiaGambetti, Elisa <1979> 28 April 2008 (has links)
The current studies assessed the role of trait anger and anger expression styles on
risk decision-making in adulthood, adolescence and childhood. In the first experiment
158 adults completed the STAXI-2 and an inventory consisting of a battery of
hypothetical everyday decision-making scenarios. Participants were also asked to
evaluate the perception of risk for each chosen option and some contextual
characteristics, that are familiarity and salience for each scenario. The study provides
evidence for a relationship between individual differences in the tendency to feel and
express anger and risky decisions and for mediation effects of familiarity and salience
appraisals. Moreover, results indicated that trait anger was predictive of risk
perception and they provide evidence for a positive relationship between risk
decision-making and risk perception.
In the second study, we examined the relationship between specific components of
anger (i.e., cognitive, affective and behavioural) and risk decision-making in
adolescents. 101 subjects completed specific tasks, measuring risk decision-making,
assessed using hypothetical choice scenarios, and anger, evaluated through the
STAXI-CA and the MSAI-R. Results showed that adolescents higher on hostility,
anger experience and destructive expression, make more risky decisions in everyday
life situations. Moreover, regression analyses indicated that destructive expression of
anger and hostility were predictive of adolescents’ risky decisions.
In the third experiment, 104 children completed three tasks: the STAXI-CA, the
MSAI-R and a task measuring risk decision-making in everyday situations. Subjects
were also asked to evaluate the degree of danger, benefit, fun and fear perceived for
each risky choice. Analyses indicated that: (a) risk decision-making was predicted by
both trait anger and outward expression of anger; (b) destructive expression o anger
was predictive of children’s risky decisions; (c) appraisal of danger fully mediated
the relation between trait anger and risk; (d) perceptions of benefit, scare and fun
partially mediated the relationship between trait anger and risk; and (e) appraisal of
danger partially mediated the relationship between outward expression of anger and
risk decision-making. The results provide evidence for a relationship between
dispositional anger and risk decision-making during childhood, suggesting a possible
explanation of the mechanisms below. In particular, risk decision-making can be
viewed as the output of cognitive and emotive processes, linked to dispositional anger
that leads children to be amused, optimistic and fearless in potentially risky
situations.
These findings substantiate the importance of incorporating cognitive and emotive
factors in theories that seek to explain the relationship between personality traits and
risk decision making across a broad range of age.
|
32 |
Effetto crowding e dislessia evolutiva: un confronto inter e intra-linguisticoBellocchi, Stéphanie <1980> 28 April 2008 (has links)
Crowding is defined as the negative effect obtained by adding visual distractors
around a central target which has to be identified. Some studies have suggested the
presence of a marked crowding effect in developmental dyslexia (e.g. Atkinson, 1991;
Spinelli et al., 2002).
Inspired by Spinelli’s (2002) experimental design, we explored the hypothesis
that the crowding effect may affect dyslexics’ response times (RTs) and accuracy in
identification tasks dealing with words, pseudowords, illegal non-words and symbolstrings.
Moreover, our study aimed to clarify the relationship between the crowding
phenomenon and the word-reading process, in an inter-language comparison
perspective. For this purpose we studied twenty-two French dyslexics and twenty-two
Italian dyslexics (total forty-four dyslexics), compared to forty-four subjects matched
for reading level (22 French and 22 Italians) and forty-four chronological age-matched
subjects (22 French and 22 Italians).
Children were all tested on reading and cognitive abilities. Results showed no
differences between French and Italian participants suggesting that performances were
homogenous. Dyslexic children were all significantly impaired in words and
pseudowords reading compared to their normal reading controls.
Regarding the identification task with which we assessed crowding effect, both
accuracy and RTs showed a lexicality effect which meant that the recognition of words
was more accurate and faster in words than pseudowords, non-words and symbolstrings.
Moreover, compared to normal readers, dyslexics’ RTs and accuracy were
impaired only for verbal materials but not for non-verbal material; these results are in
line with the phonological hypothesis (Griffiths & Snowling, 2002; Snowling, 2000;
2006) . RTs revealed a general crowding effect (RTs in the crowding condition were
slower than those recorded in the isolated condition) affecting all the subjects’
performances. This effect, however, emerged to be not specific for dyslexics. Data
didn’t reveal a significant effect of language, allowing the generalization of the
obtained results.
We also analyzed the performance of two subgroups of dyslexics, categorized
according to their reading abilities. The two subgroups produced different results
regarding the crowding effect and type of material, suggesting that it is meaningful to
take into account also the heterogeneity of the dyslexia disorder.
Finally, we also analyzed the relationship of the identification task with both
reading and cognitive abilities.
In conclusion, this study points out the importance of comparing visual tasks
performances of dyslexic participants with those of their reading level-matched
controls. This approach may improve our comprehension of the potential causal link
between crowding and reading (Goswami, 2003).
|
33 |
Scene di violenza, esperienze emotive e condotte aggressiveTosini, Giorgio <1953> 28 April 2008 (has links)
The aim of this research is to estimate the impact of violent film excerpts on university
students (30 f, 30 m) in two different sequences, a “justified” violent scene followed by an
“unjustified” one, or vice versa, as follows:
1) before-after sequences, using Aggressive behaviour I-R Questionnaire, Self Depression
Scale and ASQ-IPAT Anxiety SCALE;
2) after every excerpt, using a self-report to evaluate the intensity and hedonic tone of
emotions and the violence justification level.
Emotion regulation processes (suppression, reappraisal, self-efficacy) were considered.
In contrast with the “unjustified” violent scene, during the “justified” one, the justification
level was higher; intensity and unpleasantness of negative emotions were lower.
Anxiety (total and latent) and rumination diminished after both types of sequences.
Rumination decreases less after the JV-UV sequence than after the UV-JV sequence.
Self-efficacy in controlling negative emotions reduced rumination, whereas suppression
reduced irritability.
Reappraisal, self-efficacy in positive emotion expression and perceived emphatic selfefficacy
did not have any effects.
|
34 |
Componenti spaziali della rappresentazione cognitiva della grandezza del numeroFabbri, Marco <1979> 28 April 2008 (has links)
The humans process the numbers in a similar way to animals. There are countless
studies in which similar performance between animals and humans (adults and/or children)
are reported. Three models have been developed to explain the cognitive mechanisms
underlying the number processing. The triple-code model (Dehaene, 1992) posits an mental
number line as preferred way to represent magnitude. The mental number line has three
particular effects: the distance, the magnitude and the SNARC effects. The SNARC effect
shows a spatial association between number and space representations. In other words, the
small numbers are related to left space while large numbers are related to right space.
Recently a vertical SNARC effect has been found (Ito & Hatta, 2004; Schwarz & Keus,
2004), reflecting a space-related bottom-to-up representation of numbers. The magnitude
representations horizontally and vertically could influence the subject performance in explicit
and implicit digit tasks. The goal of this research project aimed to investigate the spatial
components of number representation using different experimental designs and tasks. The
experiment 1 focused on horizontal and vertical number representations in a within- and
between-subjects designs in a parity and magnitude comparative tasks, presenting positive or
negative Arabic digits (1-9 without 5). The experiment 1A replied the SNARC and distance
effects in both spatial arrangements. The experiment 1B showed an horizontal reversed
SNARC effect in both tasks while a vertical reversed SNARC effect was found only in
comparative task. In the experiment 1C two groups of subjects performed both tasks in two
different instruction-responding hand assignments with positive numbers. The results did not
show any significant differences between two assignments, even if the vertical number line
seemed to be more flexible respect to horizontal one. On the whole the experiment 1 seemed
to demonstrate a contextual (i.e. task set) influences of the nature of the SNARC effect. The
experiment 2 focused on the effect of horizontal and vertical number representations on
spatial biases in a paper-and-pencil bisecting tasks. In the experiment 2A the participants were
requested to bisect physical and number (2 or 9) lines horizontally and vertically. The findings
demonstrated that digit 9 strings tended to generate a more rightward bias comparing with
digit 2 strings horizontally. However in vertical condition the digit 2 strings generated a more
upperward bias respect to digit 9 strings, suggesting a top-to-bottom number line. In the
experiment 2B the participants were asked to bisect lines flanked by numbers (i.e. 1 or 7) in
four spatial arrangements: horizontal, vertical, right-diagonal and left-diagonal lines. Four
number conditions were created according to congruent or incongruent number line
representation: 1-1, 1-7, 7-1 and 7-7. The main results showed a more reliable rightward bias
in horizontal congruent condition (1-7) respect to incongruent condition (7-1). Vertically the
incongruent condition (1-7) determined a significant bias towards bottom side of line respect
to congruent condition (7-1). The experiment 2 suggested a more rigid horizontal number line
while in vertical condition the number representation could be more flexible. In the
experiment 3 we adopted the materials of experiment 2B in order to find a number line effect
on temporal (motor) performance. The participants were presented horizontal, vertical, rightdiagonal
and left-diagonal lines flanked by the same digits (i.e. 1-1 or 7-7) or by different
digits (i.e. 1-7 or 7-1). The digits were spatially congruent or incongruent with their respective
hypothesized mental representations. Participants were instructed to touch the lines either
close to the large digit, or close to the small digit, or to bisected the lines. Number processing
influenced movement execution more than movement planning. Number congruency
influenced spatial biases mostly along the horizontal but also along the vertical dimension.
These results support a two-dimensional magnitude representation. Finally, the experiment 4
addressed the visuo-spatial manipulation of number representations for accessing and retrieval
arithmetic facts. The participants were requested to perform a number-matching and an
addition verification tasks. The findings showed an interference effect between sum-nodes
and neutral-nodes only with an horizontal presentation of digit-cues, in number-matching
tasks. In the addition verification task, the performance was similar for horizontal and vertical
presentations of arithmetic problems. In conclusion the data seemed to show an automatic
activation of horizontal number line also used to retrieval arithmetic facts. The horizontal
number line seemed to be more rigid and the preferred way to order number from left-to-right.
A possible explanation could be the left-to-right direction for reading and writing. The vertical
number line seemed to be more flexible and more dependent from the tasks, reflecting
perhaps several example in the environment representing numbers either from bottom-to-top
or from top-to-bottom. However the bottom-to-top number line seemed to be activated by
explicit task demands.
|
35 |
La sorpresa come fattore critico indicatore di sintonizzazione emotiva in bambini normali e con disturbi pervasivi dello sviluppoIncasa, Iolanda <1967> 28 April 2008 (has links)
The topic of this study is surprise, re gard as an evolutionary complex process, with
manifold implication in different fields, from neurological, since aspecific correlate of
surprise exist more or less at every level of neuronal processes (e.g. Rao e Ballard,
1999.), to behavioral , inasmuch a s our ability to quickly valuate(assess), recognize
and learn from surprising events, are be regarded as pivotal for survival (e.g.
Ranganath e Rainer, 2003).
In particular this work, going from belief that surprise is really a psychoevolutive
mechanism of primary relevance, has the objective to investigate if there may be a
substantial connection between development of surprise' emotion and specific
developmental problems, or, if in subjects with pervasive developmental disorders
surprise may embody (represent) a essential mechanism of emotional tuning, and
consequently if abnormalities in such process may be at the base of at least a part of
cognitive and behavioural problems that determine (describe) this pathology.
Theoretical reasons lead us to conside r this particular pathologic condition, recall to a
broad area of research concern the comprehension of belief as marker of ability to
reasons about mental states of others (i.e. Theory of Mind), and in addition, at the
detection of specific subjects' diff iculty in this field.
On the experimental side, as well as limited of this work, we have to compare
comprehension and expression of surprise in a sample of 21 children with pervasive
developmental disorders (PDD), with a sample of 35 children without deve lopmental
problems, in a range of age 3-12.
Method
After the customary approach to become friendly with the child, an experimenter and
an accomplice showed three boxes of nuts, easily to distinguish one from the other
because of their different colours an d , working together with the child, the contents of
one of the boxes were replaced and a different material (macaroni, pebbles) was put in
the box. for the purpose of preparing a surprise for someone. At this stage, the
accomplice excused himself/herself and left and the experimenter suggested to the
child that he prepare another surprise, replacing the contents in the second box. When
the accomplice came back, the child was asked to prepare a surprise for him by
picking out the box that he thought was the right one for the purpose.
After, and the child doesn't know it, the accomplice change the content of one of the
boxes with candies and asked out to the children to open the box, in order to see if he
show surprise.
Result
Date have obtain a significant difference between autistic and normal group, in all
four tests. The expression of surprise too, is present in significantly lower degree in
autistic group than in control group. Moreover, autistic children do not provide
appropriate metarappresentative explanations.
Conclusion
Our outcome, with knowledge of the limit of our investigation at an experimental
level (low number of the champions, no possibility of video registration to firm the
expressions ) orient to consider eventuality that surprise, may be seen as relevant
component, or indicative, in autistic spectrum disorders.
|
36 |
When the others are less human: dehumanisation in intergroup relationsAlbarello, Flavia <1979> 09 April 2008 (has links)
No description available.
|
37 |
L’empowerment degli studenti universitari. Analisi del costrutto e sviluppo di uno strumento di misurazioneMarcon, Anna <1979> 09 April 2008 (has links)
No description available.
|
38 |
L'attrazione sessuale: processi psichici e strumenti di indagine. L'adattamento italiano del Cuestionario de Atraccion sexualBuizza, Chiara <1971> 27 April 2009 (has links)
The sexual attraction is a complex emotional event, with somatic modifications, that like all the emotions, belongs to the psychic dimension and that, too much simply, can be led back, from a explicative point of view, to biological and psychical causes. The sexual attraction represents a personal experience, that is difficult to record, but of which every adult is able to delineate a correspondence with own âlivedâ. In the within of biological sciences, many experimental studies were been conducted with the aim to know which chemistries substances and cerebral zones are involved in front of a stimulus assessed like sexually attractive.
Contrarily, there are little researches conducted in psychological field, aimed to the understanding the psychical factors, often unconscious, tinged to the subjective processes of sexual attraction: the sexual attraction remains an unexplored psychological event and unprovided of adapted instruments of assessment.
The present thesis is oriented in primis to examine the existing scientific literature, in order to provide an exhaustive picture of the sexual attraction and to analyze the instruments of survey available for the empirical assessment of it.
Secondly, the thesis is oriented to introduce two studies: the first study is aimed to adapt the Cuestionario de Atracción Sexual - CAS (Fernández ET to, 2006a; 2006b; 2006c) to the Italian language; the second study is aimed to assess the attitudes towards the sexual attraction in a sample of university students, in order to establish if some differences or association with some psychological dimensions (attachment stiles and alexithymia) exist.
|
39 |
Emotional engagement and orienting: the effects of picture size on affective responseDe Cesarei, Andrea <1978> 27 April 2009 (has links)
For their survival, humans and animals can rely on motivational systems which are specialized in assessing the valence and imminence of dangers and appetitive cues. The Orienting Response (OR) is a fundamental response pattern that an organism executes whenever a novel or significant stimulus is detected, and has been shown to be consistently modulated by the affective value of a stimulus. However, detecting threatening stimuli and appetitive affordances while they are far away compared to when they are within reach constitutes an obvious evolutionary advantage. Building on the linear relationship between stimulus distance and retinal size, the present research was aimed at investigating the extent to which emotional modulation of distinct processes (action preparation, attentional capture, and subjective emotional state) is affected when reducing the retinal size of a picture. Studies 1-3 examined the effects of picture size on emotional response. Subjective feeling of engagement, as well as sympathetic activation, were modulated by picture size, suggesting that action preparation and subjective experience reflect the combined effects of detecting an arousing stimulus and assessing its imminence. On the other hand, physiological responses which are thought to reflect the amount of attentional resources invested in stimulus processing did not vary with picture size. Studies 4-6 were conducted to substantiate and extend the results of studies 1-3. In particular, it was noted that a decrease in picture size is associated with a loss in the low spatial frequencies of a picture, which might confound the interpretation of the results of studies 1-3. Therefore, emotional and neutral images which were either low-pass filtered or reduced in size were presented, and affective responses were measured. Most effects which were observed when manipulating image size were replicated by blurring pictures. However, pictures depicting highly arousing unpleasant contents were associated with a more pronounced decrease in affective modulation when pictures were reduced in size compared to when they were blurred. The present results provide important information for the study of processes involved in picture perception and in the genesis and expression of an emotional response. In particular, the availability of high spatial frequencies might affect the degree of activation of an internal representation of an affectively charged scene, and might modulate subjective emotional state and preparation for action. Moreover, the manipulation of stimulus imminence revealed important effects of stimulus engagement on specific components of the emotional response, and the implications of the present data for some models of emotions have been discussed. In particular, within the framework of a staged model of emotional response, the tactic and strategic role of response preparation and attention allocation to stimuli varying in engaging power has been discussed, considering the adaptive advantages that each might represent in an evolutionary view. Finally, the identification of perceptual parameters that allow affective processing to be carried out has important methodological applications in future studies examining emotional response in basic research or clinical contexts.
|
40 |
Il rapporto tra percezione e previsione in compiti di compatibilità spazialeBazzarin, Valentina <1980> 27 April 2009 (has links)
The aim of this work is to investigate whether the actual position of a stimulus is more relevant than intuitive knowledge about the direction of an object's movement and the participants’ intention (Michaels 1988; Hommel, 1993; Proctor, Van Zandt, Lu and Weeks, 1993; Ansorge, 2000). To investigate this we used a spatial compatibility task in 4 experiments replicating what had be done by Hommel (1993) and Micheals (1988), but changing a single aspect of the procedure: neither the movement nor the effects of the action were visible to the participant. The experimental task asked them to simply imagine an action in movement.
|
Page generated in 0.0153 seconds