Spelling suggestions: "subject:"self– esteem"" "subject:"elf– esteem""
121 |
Verifying relational value: the moderating role of self-esteem in seeking self-verifying feedback.Reddoch, Lisa 10 July 2012 (has links)
People feel discomfort when they receive feedback about their relational value that is
inconsistent with their self-esteem and certainty when they receive feedback that is consistent
(Stinson et al., 2010). Feeling discomfort prompts additional feedback-seeking to confirm or
disprove the original feedback (Swann, 1987). Feeling certainty does not. People base their self-views
on years of experience and so are more likely to seek self-view consistent feedback
(Swann, 1987). Participants were given high relational value feedback to invoke discomfort in
individuals with low self-esteem (LSEs) but not individuals with high self-esteem (HSEs).
Participants were then able to seek additional relational-value feedback. LSEs were expected to
seek self-esteem consistent feedback to reduce discomfort whereas HSEs were not expected to
seek additional feedback because they would not be experiencing discomfort. Results did not
support these hypotheses for all participants: Single LSEs sought feedback as a function of self-esteem
but mated LSEs did not. / Graduate
|
122 |
Application of multilevel IRT modeling to the study of self-esteem in adolescentsPastor, Dena Ann 30 March 2011 (has links)
Not available / text
|
123 |
Depression, hopelessness, and global self-worth in a non-clinical child sampleGrinberg, Daisy I. January 1996 (has links)
The relationship between hopelessness and depression in a non-clinical sample of children (mean age = 10.75 years) was examined. A systematic comparison of three models of the role of hopelessness and global self-worth in childhood depression was conducted. The Children's Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1983), the Self-Perception Profile for Students with Learning Disabilities (Renick & Harter, 1988), and the Hopelessness Scale for Children (Kazdin, French, Unis, Esveldt-Dawson, & Sherick, 1983) were administered. Results suggest that depressed children are hopeless but that hopeless children are not necessarily depressed; global self-worth and hopelessness are highly overlapping constructs; and no gender differences are present in childhood hopelessness. Results are interpreted with reference to the theoretical implications regarding the relative support of a new theory of depression and hopelessness, versus Greene's (1989) theory of the independence of hopelessness and depression as constructs, Beck's (1967) cognitive triad theory, and Haaga, Dyck, and Ernst's(1991) single dimension model of depression.
|
124 |
Between a rock and a hard place: difficulties associated with low self-esteem in processing and responding to the romantic overtures of desirable and undesirable othersRobinson, Kelley J. 02 April 2013 (has links)
Successfully managing interpersonal relationships requires both pursuing desirable bonds and forgoing those that could be costly. Balancing these goals might be more difficult for
some than for others, especially for those with low self-esteem who are motivated to connect, yet stifled by their lack confidence in their abilities to attract desirable dating partners. So, when a potential date’s romantic interest is unambiguous, will they eagerly seize any opportunity to connect, or will the desirability of the person making the request influence their decision? In three laboratory experiments, single, female participants were randomly assigned to receive a romantic overture from an ostensible, single, male who was presented as a desirable or an undesirable dating partner. Independent of whether they accepted or rejected the target’s advances, lower, relative to higher, self-esteem individuals experienced more emotional and cognitive uncertainty and distress before and after making their decision. Desirability of the target moderated some of these effects, such that high self-esteem individuals appropriately distinguished between desirables and undesirables, whereas low self-esteem participants experienced distress at the thought of accepting or rejecting either target. Notably, the actual decisions participants made were unaffected by self-esteem, and driven instead by the extent to which the target was presented as possessing desirable social commodities. Results are discussed with reference to potential mechanisms driving self-esteem differences in balancing the pursuit of quality interpersonal bonds while avoiding costly relations.
|
125 |
Self-esteem and persistence in the face of failureDi Paula, Adam 11 1900 (has links)
Two studies tested a theory of how trait self-esteem moderates behavioral and
cognitive persistence in the face of failure. Three primary hypotheses were
examined. First, that high self-esteem (HSE) individuals persist more than low
self-esteem (LSE) individuals when their initial attempts to reach a goal fail, but
subsequent or repeated failures lead HSE individuals to reduce behavioral
persistence and pursue goal alternatives more quickly than LSE individuals.
Second, that when no goal alternatives are available, HSE individuals
behaviorally persist more than LSE individuals. Third, despite reductions in
behavioral persistence, LSE individuals do not "give up" on the failed goal but
continue to persist cognitively, in the form of aversive ruminations about the
failed goal. In a factorial experiment, persistence was examined as a function of
self-esteem, degree of failure, and the availability of goal alternatives. As
hypothesized, HSE participants behaviorally persisted more than LSE
participants after a single failure, but less after repeated failure. However, selfesteem
differences in behavioral persistence did not emerge when goal
alternatives were unavailable. Partial support was received for the hypothesis
that LSE individuals engage in more ruminative persistence than HSE
individuals-LSE participants showed higher levels of ruminative persistence on
one of two measures of ruminative persistence. The findings regarding
behavioral persistence were conceptually replicated, and the hypothesis
regarding ruminative persistence received stronger support, in a longitudinal
field study in which HSE and LSE participants initially listed their goals and reported on their behavioral and ruminative persistence regarding these goals 5
months later. HSE participants exhibited better calibration between perceptions
of goal failure and behavioral pursuit than LSE participants, indicating that
increasing perceptions of goal failure were associated with reductions in
behavioral pursuit more for HSE than LSE participants. Although they reduced
behavioral pursuit relative to HSE participants, LSE participants continued to
persist cognitively, by ruminating about their goals more than HSE participants.
Discussion focuses on the need to revise traditional views of HSE individuals
that emphasize their tenacious persistence and views of LSE individuals that
emphasize their tendency to give up in the face of failure.
|
126 |
Between a rock and a hard place: difficulties associated with low self-esteem in processing and responding to the romantic overtures of desirable and undesirable othersRobinson, Kelley J. 02 April 2013 (has links)
Successfully managing interpersonal relationships requires both pursuing desirable bonds and forgoing those that could be costly. Balancing these goals might be more difficult for
some than for others, especially for those with low self-esteem who are motivated to connect, yet stifled by their lack confidence in their abilities to attract desirable dating partners. So, when a potential date’s romantic interest is unambiguous, will they eagerly seize any opportunity to connect, or will the desirability of the person making the request influence their decision? In three laboratory experiments, single, female participants were randomly assigned to receive a romantic overture from an ostensible, single, male who was presented as a desirable or an undesirable dating partner. Independent of whether they accepted or rejected the target’s advances, lower, relative to higher, self-esteem individuals experienced more emotional and cognitive uncertainty and distress before and after making their decision. Desirability of the target moderated some of these effects, such that high self-esteem individuals appropriately distinguished between desirables and undesirables, whereas low self-esteem participants experienced distress at the thought of accepting or rejecting either target. Notably, the actual decisions participants made were unaffected by self-esteem, and driven instead by the extent to which the target was presented as possessing desirable social commodities. Results are discussed with reference to potential mechanisms driving self-esteem differences in balancing the pursuit of quality interpersonal bonds while avoiding costly relations.
|
127 |
The role of attributional styles, satisfaction with life, general health and self esteem on the psychological well being of adolescents / Jabulani Caesar MboweniMboweni, Jabulani Caesar January 2007 (has links)
The aim of the study was to determine whether adolescent psychological well being can
be significantly influenced by attributional style, general health, satisfaction with life and
self esteem. A cross-sectional research design was implemented for this study. An
availability sample of 130 learners from 2 high schools; namely Batloung and Kgobokwe
in the Ramatlabama area in rural North West Province (RSA) were chosen to collect data.
The learners were selected using the stratified random sampling technique. All
participants described themselves as equally traditional and westernised with a high value
placed on individual goals and community goals. The respondents were 12-22 years old.
Participants responded to a questionnaire consisting of six sections. The first section
comprised of biographical data. Psychological well-being was measured in terms of
distinctions between hedonism and eudaimonism. The Satisfaction With Life Scale
(SWLS) was used to measure hedonism and the PWB scale by Ryff (1995) was used to
measure eudaimonism. General health was measured by the 28 item General Health
Questionnaire (GHQ-28). The cognitive Attributional Style Questionnaire (CaQ)
measured attributional style. The I 0 item Rosenberg's Self Esteem Scale (SES) was used
to measure self esteem.
The quantitative data for the research, which was gathered through questionnaires,
was subjected to statistical analysis. This was done through the utilisation of the
Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS ver. 12). Descriptive statistics and
Cronbach alpha reliability indices were computed for each scale. Regression analysis
was used to establish the relative contribution made by the independent variables;
Attributional style (AS),Self Esteem (SE), General Health (GH) and Satisfaction With
Life (SWL); on the dependent variable (psychological well being). Analysis of Variance
(ANOVA) is used to test the significance of R2 , which is the same as testing the significance of the regression model as a whole.
The results of the study generally show that satisfaction with life, attributional styles,
self -esteem and health meaningfully predict psychological well- being (R2 = .41). the
practical significance of the finding based on the Steyn's formulation was 0.69.
psychological well-being correlated positively with satisfaction with life, general health, and the external attributions made by learners. There were no significant differences
regarding 1he nature of attributions made by both male and female learners.
It is therefore recommended that adolescents get support from parents, engage in
challenging activities, positive life events, and interact more with significant others for
their well being. Further research into the mechanisms of how life satisfaction plays its
role in positive youth development is needed to promote the psychological well-being of
all youth. / M.Soc.Sc. (Clinical Psychology) North-West University, Mafikeng Campus, 2005
|
128 |
Material possessions and identityDittmar, Helga January 1990 (has links)
No description available.
|
129 |
Participation in extracurricular activities in relation to student self-esteem levelsSchmit, Heather. January 2007 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis PlanB (M.S.)--University of Wisconsin--Stout, 2007. / Includes bibliographical references.
|
130 |
Interpersonal needs and values authenticity, belonging, independence and narcissism /Aiken, Emma. January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (DPsych) -- School of Life and Social Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, 2006. / Submitted in fulfilment of requirements for the degree of the Doctorate in Counselling Psychology, School of Life and Social Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, 2006. Includes bibliographical references (p. 233-244).
|
Page generated in 0.0744 seconds