Spelling suggestions: "subject:"focalization"" "subject:"1ocalization""
31 |
The alarm vocalizations of Brants' whistling rat, Parotomys BrantsiiLe Roux, Aliza 12 1900 (has links)
Thesis (MSc)--Stellenbosch University, 2001. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: The alarm calls of a colonial rodent, Brants' whistling rat (Parotomys brantsii), were
investigated at Goegap Nature Reserve in the Northern Cape, South Africa. At this
site P. brantsii occurs in sympatry with the congeneric P. littledalei, and the alarm
calls in response to humans was compared between the two species. Parotomys
brantsii's typical call in reaction to a human is a simple whistle, characterized by an
initial upward frequency sweep and high frequency plateau, whereas the alarm whistle
of P. littledalei has three overlapping components. Parotomys littledalei's calls are
significantly shorter and lower in dominant frequency than those of P. brantsii, and
have a significantly wider frequency bandwidth. These distinctions were attributed to
species discrimination and habitat preferences. The acoustic adaptation hypothesis is
supported in part, as P. littledalei, which inhabits a closed habitat in comparison with
P. brantsii, has calls which are lower in frequency than P. brantsii calls, but contrary
to the hypothesis, P. brantsii calls show less frequency modulation than those of P.
littledalei. The high pitch of both species' vocalizations may be a convergent feature
making the callers difficult for predators to locate.
The alarm call repertoire of P. brantsii was investigated by presenting members of the
P. brantsii colony with models of some typical predators, a raptor and puff adder, as
well as a human observer. Virtually no variation was detected in the structure of alarm
whistles elicited by different predators, indicating that P. brantsii did not identify
different predator types by means of vocalizations, even though behavioural responses
showed that P. brantsii do distinguish between different predators and non-predators.
Note duration varied in conjunction with the physical reaction of the caller. When the caller bolted towards safety, the whistle was significantly shorter than when it
remained aboveground. During an encounter with a snake or far-off human the caller
would utter relatively long duration calls, signifying a low-risk situation, while highrisk
situations such as a nearby human or raptor provoked short calls, emitted just
before the whistling rat bolted underground. I regard P. brantsii's alarm call repertoire
as a graded "urgency-based" system, indicating threat level perceived rather than
predator type. This system is widespread among ground squirrels and is usually
associated with animals inhabiting a two-dimensional environment.
Playback experiments were conducted to determine the meaning of alarm calls to
conspecifics, focusing specifically on the effect of variation in single call duration and
function of alarm-calling bouts. All alarm calls induced heightened vigilance in
receivers, but the behavioural response did not differ between short and long single
calls. Multiple calls lead to significantly longer periods of high vigilance than single
calls even after calling bouts have ended, showing that one of the functions of
multiple calls is to maintain high vigilance in conspecifics for relatively long periods.
The non-iconic nature of their alarm calls and the undiscriminating response of
receivers to different calls emphasizes the importance of contextual clues in the
communication system of animals living in an open habitat like that of P. brantsii. / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Die alarm-roepgeluide van 'n koloniale knaagdier, Brants se fluitrot (Parotomys
brantsii), is ondersoek by Goegap Natuurreservaat in the Noordkaap, Suid-Afrika,
waar hulle simpatries voorkom met die naverwante P. littledalei. Die species se
alarmroepe in reaksie op mense is met mekaar vergelyk. Die tipiese alarmroep van P.
brantsii is 'n eenvoudige fluit, gekenmerk deur 'n aanvanklike skerp
frekwensiestyging en hoë-frekwensie plato. Parotomys littledalei se fluitroep het drie
komponente wat mekaar gedeeltelike oordek. Parotomys littledalei se roepgeluide is
beduidend korter en laer in heersende toonhoogte as dié van P. brantsii, en hul
frekwensie-wydte is ook beduidend wyer. Hierdie verskille word toegeskryf aan
species-verskille en habitat-voorkeure. Die akoestiese aanpassingshipotese word
gedeeltelik deur die studie ondersteun, aangesien P. littledalei, wat 'n geslote habitat
verkies in vergelyking met P. brantsii, alarmroepe het met 'n laer dominante
toonhoogte as P. brantsii. In teenstelling met die hipotese, toon P. brantsii se
fluitgeluide minder frekwensie-modulasie as P. littledalei se geluide. Die hoë
toonhoogte van beide species se alarmroepe kan 'n konvergente verskynsel wees wat
dit vir roofdiere moeilik maak om fluitende individue te lokaliseer.
Die alarm-repertoire van P. brantsii is ondersoek deur individue bloot te stel aan 'n
menslike waarnemer en modelle van tipiese predatore, naamlik 'n jakkalsvoël en pofadder.
Daar was omtrent geen variasie in die klank-struktuur van alarmroepe in
reaksie op verskillende roofdiere nie. Dit wys daarop dat P. brantsii nie roofdiere deur
middel van roepgeluide klassifiseer nie, hoewel gedragsresponse getoon het dat
individue wel tussen roofdiere kan onderskei. Die duur van alarmroepe varieer na gelang van die gedrag van die fluitende individue - 'n fluitrot wat vlug, se roep is
beduidend korter as die geluid van een wat bogronds bly. 'n Pofadder en 'n veraf
mens ontlok lankdurende geluide, wat dui op 'n lae-risiko situasie, terwyl rotte op
vlug slaan in reaksie op 'n hoë-risiko situasie, soos met 'n nabygeleë mens of
jakkalsvoël, wanneer kort alarmroepe geuiter word. Ek beskou P. brantsii se alarmrepertoire
as 'n gegradeerde "nood-gebaseerde" stelsel, wat die mate van bedreiging
aandui, eerder as die identiteit van die spesifieke roofdier. Hierdie sisteem is tipies
van grondeekhorings en word gewoonlik verbind met diere wat 'n twee-dimensionele
omgewing bewoon.
Parotomys brantsii se reaksie op die terugspeel van die species se geluide is hierna
ondersoek. Ek het gefokus op die effek van 'n verandering in die duur van
alleenstaande fluitnote, asook hulle reaksie op fluitreekse. Alle alarmroepe het gelei
tot hoër vlakke van waaksaamheid in luisterende individue, maar reaksies op kort en
lang enkelnote het nie van mekaar verskil nie. Fluitreekse het beduidend langer
periodes van hoë waaksaamheid veroorsaak as enkele geluide, en waaksaamheid het
hoog gebly selfs nadat reekse geëindig het. Dit beteken dat fluitreekse onder andere
die funksie vervul om waaksaamheid in kolonielede vir relatief lang tye te onderhou.
Parotomys brantsii se eenvoudige alarm-repertoire en nie-onderskeidende reaksie op
verskillene roepgeluide, beklemtoon die belangrikheid van aanvullende inligting uit
die omgewing vir diere in 'n twee-dimensionele habitat wanneer die akoestiese
kommunikasiestelse slegs 'n algemene waarskuwingsfunksie vervul.
|
32 |
Geographic and species variation in bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops spp.) signature whistle typesGridley, Teresa January 2011 (has links)
Geographic variation in the whistle vocalisations of dolphins has previously been reported. However, most studies have focused on the whole whistle repertoire, with little attempt to classify sounds into biologically relevant categories. Common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) use individually distinctive signature whistles which are thought to help maintain contact between conspecifics at sea. These whistles may show a different kind of variation between populations than non-signature whistles. Here I investigate signature whistle use and variation in the two recognised species of bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus and T. aduncus) from populations inhabiting the coastal waters of the North America, Scotland, South Africa, Tanzania, Japan, Australia and New Zealand, and one captive colony. I identified likely signature whistles (signature whistles types, SWTs) from acoustic recordings by combining two novel techniques: automated contour categorisation in ARTwarp (Deecke and Janik, 2006) and a specific bout analysis based on the timing of signature whistle production in T. truncatus termed SIGID (Janik et al. in press). Three ways of categorising the contours were tested and between 87 and 111 SWTs were identified in total. Repeated emissions of stereotyped contours were apparent in the repertoire of all T. aduncus populations using both automated and human observer categorisation, providing good evidence for signature whistle use in this species. There was significant inter-specific variation in the frequency parameters, looping patterns and duration of SWTs. Inflection points, duration and measures of SWT complexity showed high variation within populations, suggesting inter- and intra-individual modification of these parameters, perhaps to enhance identity encoding or convey motivational information. Using 328 bases of the mtDNA control region, I found high levels of population differentiation (FST and φST) within the genus Tursiops. These data do not support a link between mtDNA population differentiation and variability in call type. Instead, morphological variations at the species level, and learned differences at the population level, better explain the variation found.
|
33 |
Variation in advertisement call structure of whistling frogsHay, Timothy D January 1994 (has links)
Variation in advertisement call structure of whistling frogs, Litoria ewingi, was studied among breeding sites in the vicinity of Christchurch on the East coast and Harihari on the West coast of the South Island. Males vocalized in breeding choruses throughout the year and females moved toward and appeared to evaluate calls of territorial males before initiating amplexus. Acoustic interactions between neighbours led to modification of call structure and timing, and wrestling matches between males were frequent. I quantified call structure for 1623 calls of 168 individuals using 24 call parameters. Most temporal characteristics were significantly correlated with the caller's body temperature and dominant frequency was negatively correlated with body size. These results are consistent with the prediction of temperature-dependent metabolic rates in a poikilotherm's nervous system. I compared call structure among the study populations using both multivariate and univariate analyses. Advertisement call structure showed highly significant variation among populations for most call variables. The greatest difference occurred between the West and East coast populations, and a clinal trend occurred in the East coast populations on a microgeographic scale. Variation among East coast populations in some variables was comparable to the scale of variation between coasts. These results suggest that whistling frogs exhibit natal philopatry. I obtained repeated recordings for 21 individuals and examined variation in call structure within and among individuals using nested multivariate analyses of variance. Highly significant variation occurred both within and among individuals for most call characters, but certain call characters were more variable among individuals. This suggests that whistling frog advertisement calls are individually distinctive.
|
34 |
Effect of age on laryngeal airway resistance during vowel productionMelcon, Matthew Clinton, 1957- January 1988 (has links)
An investigation was conducted to determine if laryngeal valving economy, as reflected in measures of laryngeal airway resistance during vowel production, varies across adulthood. Toward this end, sixty healthy men were studied, ten from each of six age groups--25, 35, 45, 55, 65, and 75 years (± 2 years). Results showed that there are age-related differences in laryngeal airway resistance during vowel production, and that these differences are characterized by a lower mean resistance in 75-year-old men than in younger men of the ages studied. This finding provides insight into mechanisms regarding the impact of age on laryngeal function and has important clinical implications for the evaluation and management of men with voice disorders.
|
35 |
Correlates and temporal variation in call phenology of eastern Ontario frogsKLAUS, SAMANTHA P 14 November 2012 (has links)
Climate change has been predicted to have long-term consequences for North American ecosystems. Amphibians in particular are considered indicators of ecosystem health because of their sensitivity to environmental change – due in part to their semi-permeable skin and aquatic developmental requirements. Amphibians have been experiencing population declines on a global scale, suggested in part to be due to changes in reproductive behaviour and timing. My thesis examines the abiotic correlates of timing of calling in southeastern Ontario anurans, as well as the extent to which the timing of calling events vary within and among species. I focused on eight species of frogs using both a 40-year historical dataset and a 5-year field-collected dataset of environmental variation and anuran activity. From analysis of the historical dataset, Lithobates pipiens was the only species out of eight to emerge significantly earlier, by an estimated 22 days over four decades. Both L. pipiens and Anaraxyus americanus have advanced initiation of calling over a four-decade span significantly earlier by an estimated 37.2 and 19.2 days, respectively, correlating with significant regional increases in spring air temperatures (2.8°C over four decades). Global frog declines or range shifts relate ultimately to changes in reproductive behaviour and timing mediated by shifting climate. From my analysis of the field-collected dataset, I concluded that species varied in the environmental predictors that best predicted this variation may be a consequence of varying reproductive strategies between early spring “explosive” breeders versus late prolonged breeders. There was also significant among-location variation in calling activity for four prolonged breeding species, which may be an effect of significant microclimatic variation between locations surveyed. My study suggests that local temperature increases have affected the timing of emergence and the onset of calling activity in some frogs and that microclimatic differences among breeding habitats may be influencing the timing of breeding in some prolonged breeding species. My research aids future conservation and management strategies for North America’s dwindling amphibian populations by quantifying how abiotic factors influence breeding behaviour on both a fine and extended temporal scale as well as by developing and testing standardized methods for long-term species monitoring. / Thesis (Master, Biology) -- Queen's University, 2012-11-12 23:05:34.12
|
36 |
Acoustic Properties of Early Vocalizations in Infants With Fragile X SyndromeLisa M. Rague (5930804) 03 January 2019 (has links)
Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is a neurogenetic syndrome characterized by cognitive impairments and high rates of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). FXS is often used as a model for exploring mechanisms and pathways of symptom expression in ASD due to the high prevalence of ASD in this population and the known single-gene cause for ASD in FXS. Early vocalization features – including volubility, canonical complexity, vocalization duration and vocalization pitch – have shown promise in detecting ASD in idiopathic ASD populations but have yet to be extensively studied in a population with a known cause for ASD, such as FXS. The present study characterizes early vocalization features in FXS, demonstrating how these features are associated with language ability and ASD outcomes, as well as highlighting how these features in FXS may diverge from patterns observed in typically developing (TD) populations. We coded vocalization features during a standardized child-examiner interaction in 39 nine-month-old infants (22 FXS, 17 TD) who were then followed up at 24 months to determine developmental and clinical outcomes. Although many findings did not reach statistical significance in this small sample, our results provide preliminary evidence that infants with FXS may demonstrate patterns of associations with 24-month language outcomes that diverge from those observed in typical development, and that certain vocalization features may be associated with later ASD outcomes in the FXS group. These findings warrant more research exploring these features as potential early markers of ASD in FXS. Characterizing the associations of early vocalization features with ASD outcomes in FXS can inform mechanisms of ASD development that can then be tested broadly with other etiologically-distinct populations at risk for ASD. Thus, further characterization of these early vocalization features in typical and atypical development may lead to improved early identification methods, treatment approaches, and overall well-being of individuals in the ASD population.
|
37 |
INTRASEXUAL SELECTION AND THE ELABORATION OF A MATING SIGNAL IN THE BACHMAN’S SPARROW (PEUCEA AESTIVALIS)Unknown Date (has links)
Vocal communication is central to the coordination of social behavior in many vertebrate species, and it has been particularly well studied in songbirds, which use their songs in different contexts to convey information about the singer or its environment (Catchpole and Slater 2008; Alger et al. 2016). While it is widely accepted that the songs of oscine passerine birds (the songbirds) have two main social functions: intrasexual competition, and courtship (Catchpole and Slater 2008), the evolution of large and complex song repertoires remains an evolutionary puzzle (Byers and Kroodsma 2009). The question is: why do some songbird species produce an elaborate vocal repertoire, while other species carry out courtship and competition with a far smaller and simpler repertoire? In this thesis I examine the adaptive value (social function) of song in malemale competition with an eye toward understanding how intrasexual selection may have driven the elaboration of the male Bachman’s sparrow (Peucea aestivalis) vocal repertoire. / Includes bibliography. / Thesis (M.S.)--Florida Atlantic University, 2019. / FAU Electronic Theses and Dissertations Collection
|
38 |
The functions and mechanisms of predator-associated vocalisations in North American red squirrels (Tamiasciurus Hudsonicus)Digweed, Shannon Marie, University of Lethbridge. Faculty of Arts and Science January 2009 (has links)
North American red squirrels are a solitary, territorial species that confront various
predators. Previous research proposed that squirrels produce predator-specific, referential
‘seet’ and ‘bark’ alarm calls to aerial and ground predators, respectively. To test this
hypothesis, I examined alarm call production during natural encounters with predators,
conspecific intruders and in a series of predator simulation experiments. Call production
patterns were consistent across all types of disturbance and involved protracted bouts
where both call types were inter-mixed. Hence, the call types were not predator-specific
but rather their patterning reflected the persistence of disturbances of any type. Tests of
alternative call functions further indicated that calls were not actually directed at
conspecifics, but rather at predators and intruders and might function to deter or repel
them directly. These outcomes are consistent with life-history details of red squirrels and
contradict the proposal that this species produces predator-specific, referential alarm
calls. / xi, 153 leaves : ill. ; 29 cm
|
39 |
Vocalization behavior of captive loggerhead shrikes (Lanius ludovicianus excubitorides)Soendjoto, Mochamad Arief January 1995 (has links)
Vocalization behavior of captive loggerhead shrikes was studied at the Avian Science and Conservation Centre of McGill University. In the first stage, calls of two pairs kept in indoor cages were individually recorded not only to catalogue these calls spectrographically and quantitatively but also to use them in identifying the birds sexually. Males vocalized 21 call figures for 16.50% of the observation time and females, 8 call figures for 2.64% of the observation time. Males contiguously delivered 1 to 11 bouts with a mean of 7.25 min for a rate of 6.06 bouts/h; females delivered 1 to 9 bouts with a mean of 4.07 min for a rate of 1.27 bouts/h. Males not only vocalized at a higher rate and longer than females, but also demonstrated trill calls which the females did not do. In the second stage, five pairs were paired in large outdoor breeding pens. Their calls were recorded and current visual displays observed in an effort to understand calls related to breeding behaviors. Two new calls figures vocalized by males as well as 2 call figures by young shrikes were recorded. Each male demonstrated distinctive calls that differed from those of other males during nest-site selection, nest building and copulation, but similar calls during food offering, aggressive and alarm behavior. Despite the call differences, all males performed similar visual displays during the above activities. Conversely, breeding females gave no calls, other than harsh calls during food offering, food begging, aggressive and alarm behaviors.
|
40 |
Escape and vocal responses of eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus) to simulated aerial predator attackBonenfant, Marjolaine. January 1996 (has links)
The behaviours of eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus) in response to aerial predators were studied in the field using trained kestrels (Falco sparverius) flying over a food patch where animals of known gender, age and burrow location categories were foraging. Their typical response was to flee toward a nearby refuge while producing a trill. After a few seconds, chipmunks usually emerged and started to produce long (but sometimes interrupted) series of chucks while facing the predator. Acoustical analysis showed that the trill consisted of a rapid series of usually high pitched and variable notes while most chucks consisted of two partially overlapping components differing mainly in frequency range. No differences were observed between individuals of different categories for most antipredator responses. Various observations suggest that the trill functions to startle the predator and that the chuck deters the predator from hunting in the area.
|
Page generated in 0.0729 seconds