• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 3843
  • 439
  • 422
  • 309
  • 260
  • 198
  • 76
  • 61
  • 58
  • 57
  • 56
  • 41
  • 38
  • 38
  • 34
  • Tagged with
  • 7368
  • 2867
  • 1254
  • 615
  • 553
  • 546
  • 529
  • 527
  • 527
  • 515
  • 475
  • 423
  • 415
  • 403
  • 376
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
271

Consumer evaluations of brand alliances

James, David Owen January 2000 (has links)
No description available.
272

The validity and utility of computer based test interpretations (CBTIs) in staff selection decision situations

Rolls, Stevan Richard January 1993 (has links)
No description available.
273

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CHARACTER TYPE AND COGNITIVE-MORAL DEVELOPMENT

Hasler, Joseph Francis, 1958- January 1987 (has links)
This study investigated the relationship between the character typology proposed by C. G. Jung, and the stages of cognitive-moral development described by Lawrence Kohlberg. It was hypothesized that certain character types, particularly those preferring introversion and intuition, would display the higher levels of moral development. A total of 120 male and female introductory psychology students at the University of Arizona were administered the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) to assess character type, the Sociomoral Reflection Objective Measure (SROM) to identify stage of moral development, and the Shipley-Hartford Institute of Living Scale as a brief estimate of IQ. The only significant differences in moral development as a function of character type were between the judging and perceiving types, with perceiving types displaying the higher mean SROM score (p .02). This finding suggests that open-mindedness is more conducive to moral development than the tendency to jump to immediate conclusions.
274

Reversal theory and psychotherapy

Murgatroyd, S. J. January 1987 (has links)
No description available.
275

Psychosocial factors in breast cancer

Cheang, Amy W. H. January 1984 (has links)
No description available.
276

The development of a computerised package to produce cumulative reliabilities based on the approximation to Cronbach's Alpha and to applied this package to personality and attitude scales

Woods, A. January 1988 (has links)
No description available.
277

The sociological problem of personality formation : with special reference to symbolic interactionist, Marxist and figurational approaches

Burkitt, Ian January 1989 (has links)
No description available.
278

Population studies of variation in catecholamine and corticosteroid excretion

Jenner, D. A. January 1985 (has links)
No description available.
279

Identifying faking on self-report personality inventories: Relative merits of traditional lie scales, new lie scales, response patterns, and response times

LAMBERT, CHRISTINE ELIZABETH 28 September 2013 (has links)
The use of personality tests throughout Canadian society is based on the assumption that their results are valid. However, research has shown that individuals can, and do, fake their responses on personality inventories. Individuals may fake good, emphasizing their positive characteristics, or fake bad, emphasizing negative characteristics, in order to obtain a desired outcome. Recent research has provided support for a congruence model of faking, which states that schema-consistent responses are provided more quickly than schema-inconsistent responses. Faking successfully, without being detected by validity indices, requires balancing favourable and unfavourable responses, regardless of the faking schema a participant adopts. This demand results in cognitive fatigue over time, producing increasingly unbalanced response patterns. Two studies were conducted to evaluate the efficacy of the congruence and cognitive overload models of faking in detecting instructed faking, and to examine whether these models or the newly developed Faking Response Strategy Scales provide added value in detecting faking relative to currently established gold-standard measures. Results showed that all of the self-report scales examined—whether traditional or new—were valid detectors of faking, which supports their ongoing use. However, results highlighted the weakness of the Impression Management subscale of the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding, the current gold-standard in the field, in providing added value relative to other scales. Response latency data supported the congruence model of faking, but results for the cognitive overload model were mixed: Study 1 data supported the cognitive overload model, but time constraints introduced in Study 2 seem to have caused random responding, rather than increasing cognitive overload as was intended. Results supported a multidimensional model of faking, and show that adding measures of response latency and response pattern can enhance the ability of traditional measures to detect faking. These findings have important theoretical and practical implications for methods of detecting faking and for the understanding of cognitive processes underlying faking. / Thesis (Master, Psychology) -- Queen's University, 2013-09-28 11:00:11.347
280

A study to investigate the psychosocial characteristics of women with chronic pelvic pain

Regan, Joanne Cathryn January 1997 (has links)
No description available.

Page generated in 0.0525 seconds