• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 316
  • 34
  • 30
  • 30
  • 30
  • 30
  • 30
  • 30
  • 19
  • 16
  • 11
  • 10
  • 9
  • 8
  • 7
  • Tagged with
  • 562
  • 562
  • 115
  • 81
  • 62
  • 62
  • 62
  • 46
  • 38
  • 36
  • 34
  • 34
  • 29
  • 29
  • 29
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
71

Genetic and functional characterization of the Piwi proteins and piRNAs of Caenorhabditis elegans

Bagijn, Marloes Pauline January 2011 (has links)
No description available.
72

The miR-51 family of miRNAs is required for the maintenance of pharyngeal attachment in Caenorhabditis elegans

Shaw, William Robert January 2010 (has links)
No description available.
73

Regulation of embryonic and postembryonic cell divisions in Caenorhabditis elegans

Kostić, Ivana January 2002 (has links)
To understand the molecular basis of developmental control of cell division during C. elegans organogenesis, two different approaches were taken. First, a screen was performed to identify mutants with altered numbers of intestinal nuclei using a reporter transgene specific to the intestinal nuclei. The intestine displays three different cell division patterns; mitosis, karyokinesis and endoreplication, therefore, in this screen we could potentially isolate mutants in genes affecting any of these different cell cycles. An F2 semi-clonal screen was performed and mutants with fewer or supernumerary numbers of intestinal nuclei were isolated. One mutant, rr31, with twice the wild type complement of intestinal nuclei was mapped and the defect was subsequently shown to be due to a gain-of-function mutation in the cell cycle phosphatase cdc-25.1. Further characterization of the cdc-25.1(gf) mutant, showed that the extra intestinal cells arise from an additional division of the intestinal cell precursors during embryogenesis, and that this phenotype is unique to the intestinal lineage. (Abstract shortened by UMI.)
74

Phenotypic and molecular analysis of the maternal effect associated with mutations in the clk-1 gene of Caenorhabditis elegans

Burgess, Jason. January 2002 (has links)
Mutations in the Caenorhabditis elegans maternal-effect gene clk-1 result in a highly pleiotropic phenotype, characterized by a general slow down in embryonic and larval development, as well as a slowing down of adult behaviors including defecation, pharyngeal pumping and swimming. First generation homozygous clk-1 mutants descended from a heterozygous mother are fully rescued for these mutant phenotypes. It has been shown that CLK-1 protein is a hydroxylase that acts in the conversion of demethoxyubiquinone (DMQ) to 5-hydroxyubiquinone, in the ubiquinone (Q) biosynthesis pathway. Consequently, clk-1 mutants accumulate the Q9 precursor, DMQ9 (the subscript refers to the length of the isoprenoid side chain). Here, I show that the profound maternal rescue observed in clk-1 maternally rescued animals is due to presence of the CLK-1 protein throughout larval development, in sufficient amounts to catalyze the production of Q9. clk-1 mutants have been shown to have a dietary requirement for Q8 due to their inability to synthesize Q9. I demonstrate that clk-1 maternally rescued animals have sufficient amounts of Q 9 to complete larval development and produce an almost full brood when raised on a Q8 deficient E. coli strain. I also show that prolonged arrest at the first larval stage, which is likely to result in degradation of any maternally contributed mRNA or protein, brings about a Clk mutant phenotype in maternally rescued animals. Finally, I reveal that the Clk mutant phenotype can be rescued at any larval stage by ectopic expression of CLK-1, suggesting that there is no developmental window for the rescue of clk-1 mutants by CLK-1. These results identify perdurance of maternally contributed product throughout development as the mechanism that accounts for the maternal effect observed in clk-1 mutants.
75

Mitochondrial import and localization of CLK-1 in Caenorhabditis elegans

Ubach, Antonio. January 2001 (has links)
Several classes of genes determine the lifespan of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Our laboratory is particularly interested in the clk class of genes that is composed of clk-1, clk-2, clk-3 , and gro-1. Mutations in these genes have been shown to extend lifespan and to deregulate several developmental and behavioural processes such as pharyngeal pumping and defecation cycle length. / clk-1 encodes a 187 amino acid mitochondrial protein that is composed of two homologous TRC domains (TRC for T&barbelow;andemly R&barbelow;epeated in C&barbelow;LK-1). Interestingly, the yeast homologue of clk-1, COQ7, has been implicated in ubiquinone biosynthesis. clk-1 (e2519) lesion is a point mutation that changes a conserved amino acid (E148K) in the second TRC domain. A structural model proposed that clk-1 is a di-iron carboxylate protein. We found that mutations in the di-iron binding center abolish CLK-1 activity and modify the subcellular distribution of CLK-1 in clk-1( qm30) null mutants.
76

Factors affecting long-term habituation in Caenorhabditis elegans

Beck, Christine Daily O’Brien 11 1900 (has links)
The objective of these experiments was to explore long-term memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. This examination of memory in a simple organism with accessible genetics and a well understood biology may permit later work to define the cellular processes that underlie long-term memory. Habituation training with a vibrational stimulus was administered on Day 1, and the retention test of a block of stimuli was given 24 h after the end of training on Day 2. Long-term retention of habituation was evident as a lower level of responding on Day 2 relative to the level of responding on Day 2 of untrained controls or the initial level of responding of worms on Day 1. In Experiments 1 and 2, a habituation training protocol that produced long-term retention of habituation was established, and the effects of stimulus number, interstimulus interval (ISI), and distribution of training on both short-term and long-term habituation were examined. In Experiment 1 (10-s ISI), there appeared to be a floor effect which resulted in a low level of responding regardless of training on Day 1; thus no evidence for long-term habituation after training at a 10-s ISI could be found. In Experiment 2 (60-s ISI), worms that received distributed and massed habituation training with 60 stimuli showed a significantly lower level of responding relative to untrained controls. The distributed habituation training appeared to be more effective at inducing long-term habituation and was used in the subsequent experiments. To characterize the effects of heat shock treatments used in the behavioral experiments that follow, the effects of heat shock on two assays, the induction of a heat shock protein gene, hsp16, and the rate of egg-laying were measured in Experiment 3. All heat shock treatments used caused the induction of hsp16. In addition, the number of eggs laid during a fixed interval after heat shock was sensitive to the heat shock treatments given in Experiments 4 through 8. In Experiments 4 through 8, the effects of heat shock on short- and long-term habituation were examined. Heat shock, which acts as a general cellular stressor, was administered at different times before, during and after training. In Experiment 4, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered, ending 2 h before training on Day 1. Heat shock before training did not affect the initial level of responding on Day 1, habituation during training, short-term retention of habituation between blocks of training or long-term retention of habituation. In Experiment 5, heat shock (45 min, 32°C) was administered during the rest periods of distributed training in the 1-h interval after each training block. While heat shock during training had no significant effect on responding on Day 1, long-term habituation was blocked. In Experiment 6, the possibility that heat shock before training would prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training by inducing thermal tolerance was examined. This was tested by administering heat shock (45 min, 32°C) that ended 2 h before training and heat shock during training. It was found that heat shock before training did not prevent the disruption of long-term habituation by heat shock during training. In Experiment 7, the effect of heat shock that ended 2 h before the retention test on Day 2 on the retention of long-term habituation was examined. It was found that heat shock on Day 2 did not disrupt the retention of habituation. Finally, in Experiment 8, the effect of brief heat shock (15 min, 32°C) at different intervals in the rest period following the training blocks was examined in an attempt to more narrowly define a critical period for consolidation of long-term habituation. Although there was no significant effect of brief heat shock on retention of habituation, the pattern of the data suggests that there may be a period of greater vulnerability worth further investigation. In summary, heat shock given before training or before the retention test did not affect long-term habituation, while heat shock during training disrupted consolidation of long-term habituation. Taken together, these behavioral results provide the foundation for an investigation of the cellular processes underlying long-term memory in C. elegans. By exploring the dynamics of the formation of long-term habituation, intervals of time critical to the formation of long-term habituation were defined. This in turn will help to focus attention on the cellular processes whose activity during those intervals of time may be important to the consolidation of long-term memory.
77

Evaluation of caenorhabditis elegans as an acute lethality and a neurotoxicity screening model

Williams, Phillip Lindly 12 1900 (has links)
No description available.
78

Development and evaluation of toxicity tests using Caenorhabditis elegans with reproduction, mutation, lethality, and behavior as end points

Middendorf, Paul Joseph 05 1900 (has links)
No description available.
79

Threshold chemosensitivity of the nematode caenorhabditis elegans

Terrill, William Forrest 05 1900 (has links)
No description available.
80

A soil toxicity test using the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans and some applications to studying metal ion sorption processes in soils

Donkin, Steven Glenn 08 1900 (has links)
No description available.

Page generated in 0.085 seconds