• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 239
  • 51
  • 34
  • 34
  • 34
  • 34
  • 34
  • 34
  • 20
  • 10
  • 7
  • 7
  • 6
  • 2
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 495
  • 495
  • 121
  • 114
  • 79
  • 66
  • 54
  • 54
  • 50
  • 44
  • 44
  • 43
  • 42
  • 41
  • 41
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
281

A comparison of children's ability to apply morphological inflections to nonsense and English lexical words /

Le Gallais, Judy January 1977 (has links)
No description available.
282

The effect of study trips followed by discussion on vocabulary development of kindergarten pupils

Kraus, Lydia 01 January 1970 (has links)
It was the purpose of this study to determine the effect of study trips and subsequent discussion on the vocabulary of kindergarten pupils from three social groups in Madora County. Seventy-nine pupils, selected from three classrooms in three rural school districts, were classified as middle-class Anglo-American, lower-class Anglo-American, and lower-class Mexican-American. Two of the three classes were designated as experimental and one as the control class. The instructional design, twelve study trips (one each week) and ensuing discussions, encourage pupils to use new words and develop and expand the meanings of words. The major source of data was the Watts Vocabulary Test for Young Children administered prior to and following the experimental instruction. To provide a basis for interpreting the vocabulary data, the language skills of the pupils were rated by their teachers. Nineteen pupils, selected as high, average, and low verbal responders, were observed in the classroom to determine the effect of the instruction on their verbal behavior. Data also were collected, by the use of observation techniques, to find the extent the instructional plan was implemented and its effect on the verbal behavior of all pupils. An analysis of variance technique was applied to the means obtained on the Watts test to determine the statistical relationship of the experimental and control groups and of the social-class groups. Significant differences, at the .05 level, were located by using post hoc tests. The t test was used to find differences between means. A correlation technique was used to compare vocabulary scores and the language ratings. Language ratings and social-class placements were compared with the quartile distribution of scores for the nineteen pupils selected for more intensive observation. The conclusions from the study include the following: (1) the vocabularies of the pupils in each of the social groups in the experimental classes were effectively stimulated by the instruction; (2) the instruction was most effective for the middle-class pupils, and the least effective for the lower-class Mexican-American pupils; (3) in the distribution of language ratings and vocabulary scores the Mexican-American pupils usually placed low, middle-class Anglo pupils high, and lower-class Anglo pupils in the middle. The findings suggest the needs for further research to: (1) validate the instructional plan by replicating the study in the same school districts; (2) evaluate the effect of the instruction on vocabulary skill after an interval of two years; (3) measure the results of similar curricula administered to the same pupils for at least three years; (4) determine the gain in English vocabulary when the discussions include the use of the native language of the Mexican-American pupils; (5) evaluate the effects of the plan when used with younger pupils.
283

The relationship of language orientation and racial/ethnic attitude among Chinese-American primary grade children

Kwok, Irene Sui-ling 01 January 1979 (has links)
This study to determine whether there was a relationship between the language orientation of Chinese-American primary-grade children in the San Francisco Unified School District and their racial/ethnic attitude. Generally, Chinese-Americans are monolingual Chinese speakers (MCS), monolingual English speakers (BECS). These three linguistic groups of Chineses-American children were compared in this study to determine the existence of a language orientation-racial/ethnic attitude relationship. A review of the literature shows that 1) language, thought, and perception are interrelated, 2) racial attitudes are significant factors in American society, 3) attitudes and behavior are interrelated, and 4) children develop racial/ethnic attitudes at an early age. The problem incorporates each of these areas, while focusing on the dependent variable of racial/ethnic attitude.
284

The Relationship Between One and Five-Minute Agpar Scores and Linguistic Functioning as Measured by the Test of Language Development

Smith, Elizabeth W. 01 July 1982 (has links) (PDF)
No description available.
285

On some possible etiological mechanisms of developmental dysphasia

Gurd, J. M. (Jennifer Mary) January 1984 (has links)
No description available.
286

An exploration of developmental reciprocal communication in the dialogue journals of third graders

Veltri, Mildred Middlemiss 19 October 2005 (has links)
Dialogue journal writing is a form of reciprocal communication in which pupils and teachers inform, and react to the entries of, the partner in a nonevaluative, familiar dialogue. Journals were analyzed in terms of three dependent variables: detail, response complexity, and reference. To study the relationship of journal writing to other characteristics, three independent variables were assessed-- audience awareness, by an adaptation of Braig's (1984) audience awareness categories; social cognition, by Miller, Kessel & Flavell's (1970) assessment of social cognitive development; and writing ability, by an evaluation developed by McCaig (1984). Entries from the dialogue journals of 21 3rd grade students were rated at 3 times--10 consecutive from the beginning, middle, and end of the year. Questions asked were: 1. Do dialogue journals have separate, unique elements, or do they have a single domain? 2. Is there a relationship between the dependent and independent measures? 3. Does skill increase over time for the dependent variables? 4. What is the effect of gender, use of English as a second language, or minority status over time for each of the dependent variables? Spearman rho correlations addressed the first and second questions. Stepwise regression analyses was also completed. Question three was investigated using repeated measures ANOVA. Repeated measures first order interactions and between-subjects differences on dialogue journal scales were used to answer the fourth question. Results of this study indicate that at there are at least three relatively independent components for dialogue journal writing with a large proportion of the variance in detail related to audience awareness and social cognition, and somewhat less to writing ability. There were significant time effects for response complexity and reference. Detail increased, but not significantly. There were no significant first order interactions with time for any of the three demographic variables. However, between-subject differences on dialogue journal scales suggest directions for further study of group and individual differences in dialogue journal writing as reciprocal communication. / Ed. D.
287

Acquisition of Cantonese passive bei2 constructions by deaf children.

January 2012 (has links)
早期口語習得研究假設被動句的賓語通過“論元移位“ (A-movement) 移到主語位置。Huang (1999)和 Tang (2000, 2001) 認為粤語被動句和漢語被動句類似,都通過空運算元移位形成,即空運算元通過“非論元移位“(A’-movement) 從內嵌 VP的賓語位置移到時態短語(TP)指定語的位置。經過謂語化後,這個空語類受主語約束。 / 漢語被動句習得研究顯示兒童較遲獲得長被動句 (Chang, 1986; Xu and Yang, 2008; Liu and Ning, 2009)。粤語表被動的「畀」存在一詞多義形式,除了表示被動,還有表示給予的「畀」,表示與格的「畀」,以及表示允許的「畀」。Wong (2004) 發現表被動的「畀」最難習得。 / 本研究從普遍語法 (UG)的角度來考察粤語「畀」字被動句的獲得情況,旨在探討聾童如何習得粤語「畀」字被動句,他們是否經歷與健聽兒童相似的習得過程。同時,進一步研究聾童是否掌握粤語「畀」字被動句內部的語法關係,包括基本的句法語序,內嵌使役結構的有終結性“telic“,以及空運算元和主句主語的約束關係。本研究採用了三種實驗方法:圖片選擇、圖片描述,以及圖片判斷。參與此項研究的被試有 18名 7;03至 12;06之間的聾童,40名 3;03至 7;05之間的健聽兒童,以及作為控制組的 20名健聽成人。 / 圖片選擇和圖片描述實驗的結果表明聾童和健聽兒童在粤語「畀」字被動句的發展中經歷了相似的習得過程。他們表達含光杆動詞的被動句沒有什麼困難。光杆動詞,比如“踢“,若自身帶有“終結體“(+telic) 的特徵,在粤語被動句中是合語法的。但是,當需要使用動補複合動詞 (RVC) 來表達一個事件的有終結性特徵時,聾童與健聽兒童的表現則不同。聾童很難以這種有終結性的特徵解讀這個內嵌的使役結構。取而代之,他們保留用光杆動詞結構來表達單單一個動作或一個結果。亦有不少聾童使用兩個連接的短語,以前一個表達動作,後一個表達結果。圖片判斷實驗的結果顯示,隨著“語言接收年齡“(receptive language age) 的提高,聾童對可加強被動義的標記「親」(-can1)意識也逐漸提高。在「畀」字句中,含被動義的標記「親」用於限制: (a) 句子在表允許和表被動的歧義,使句子解讀為被動義; (b) 主句主語和內嵌賓語的約束關係。 / 為深入瞭解聾童「畀」字被動句的獲得,本研究同時考察了他們能否區別「畀」字的一詞多義形式及其相應的句法結構,包括表給予的「畀」和表允許的「畀」。研究結果顯示「畀」字被動句帶有兩個小句,且含空運算元移位,這給聾童被動句的習得造成了極大的困難。儘管表允許的「畀」也帶有兩個小句,含賓語控制結構,相對而言仍比被動句容易習得。表給予的「畀」為單句,我們假設應發展得較早,但是在本研究中卻發現比表允許的「畀」更遲習得。事實上,聾童在產出粤語 IDOC (即 [N bei2 DO IO], DO 代表直接賓語而 IO代表間接賓語) 語序時有困難,可是卻會採用漢語語法的語序 DOC (即 [N bei2 IO DO])。我們認為這可能是漢語書面語的輸入對聾童更為“可接收 (accessible)所致;相比之下,由於聽力受損,他們通過聽覺接收的粤語口語輸入卻受到了限制。 / Earlier acquisition studies of passives in spoken languages assume A-movement of the object to the subject position. Huang (1999) and Tang (2000, 2001) claim that Cantonese passives, similar to Mandarin passives, involve null operator movement from the object position of the embedded VP via A’movement to the specifier position of the intermediate TP. This null category is then bound by a base-generated subject through predication. / Acquisition studies of Mandarin passives show that children acquire long passives late (Chang, 1986; Xu and Yang, 2008; Liu and Ning, 2009). As for Cantonese, Wong (2004) found that passive bei2 is most difficult among the different polysemous bei2, namely transfer bei2 ‘give’, dative bei2, and permissive bei2 ‘let’. / In the current study, we approach the acquisition of Cantonese passive bei2 from a UG perspective. We aim to see how deaf children acquire passive bei2, whether they go through a similar acquisition process as their typical hearing counterparts. We also want to see if they have developed knowledge about the grammatical relations involved with passive bei2, namely the basic syntactic order, the telicity encoded in the embedded causative structure, and the binding relationship between the null operator and the matrix subject. Three experimental procedures were developed: a picture selection task, a picture description task and a picture verification task. 18 deaf children (7;03-12;06), 40 younger hearing children (3;03-7;05) and 20 hearing adults as controls participated in this study. / The results in the picture selection and the picture description task show that the deaf children and the hearing children went through a similar acquisition process in the development of passive bei2. They did not seem to encounter difficulty in producing passives bei2 involving a bare verb which is inherently +telic such as ‘kick’, which is acceptable in Cantonese. However, when coming to using a resultative verb compound (RVC) to encode the +telic aspect of the eventuality, the deaf subjects’ performance differed from the hearing subjects in demonstrating difficulty in encoding a telic causative event structure. Instead, they maintained a bare verb structure to encode either an activity or a result. A number of deaf children also used two conjoined phrases, with the first encoding an activity and the second a result. The results of the picture verification task show that as a function of receptive language age, the deaf children in our study were sensitive to the presence of the adversative marker -can1, which is used to restrict (a) the interpretation of an ambiguous permissive/passive sentence to a passive reading, and (b) the binding relation between the matrix subject and the embedded object involved. / To deepen our understanding of deaf children’s acquisition of passive bei2, part of the project is to examine if they could identify the other polysemous bei2s, including transfer bei2 and permissive bei2 as they involve different linguistic structures. The results show that passive bei2 which requires a biclausal structure plus null operator movement poses as the most difficult construction to be acquired by deaf children. Although permissive bei2 also invokes a biclausal structure with object control, it is relatively easier than passive bei2. Unexpectedly, transfer bei2 is found to be more difficult than permissive bei2 although the structure is monoclausal and assumed to be developmentally early in typically developed children. In fact, the deaf subjects have experienced difficulty in producing the IDOC order (i.e. [N bei2 DO IO], where DO represents Direct Object and IO represents Indirect Object), a required word order in Cantonese; instead, they resorted to the DOC (i.e. [N bei2 IO DO]) order which reflects the Mandarin grammar. We propose that it is due to the more ‘accessible’ input from printed written Chinese, which follows the Mandarin grammar, as against the less accessible Cantonese input through the auditory mode. / Detailed summary in vernacular field only. / Detailed summary in vernacular field only. / Detailed summary in vernacular field only. / Detailed summary in vernacular field only. / Detailed summary in vernacular field only. / Yiu, Kun Man. / Thesis (M.Phil.)--Chinese University of Hong Kong, 2012. / Includes bibliographical references (leaves 232-242). / Abstracts also in Chinese; appendix 1 includes Chinese. / Abstract --- p.ii / Acknowledgement --- p.vi / Table of Contents --- p.viii / List of Figures --- p.xiii / List of Tables --- p.xvi / Abbreviations --- p.xvii / Chapter Chapter 1 --- Introduction --- p.1 / Chapter 1.1. --- Introduction --- p.1 / Chapter 1.2. --- Spoken Language Acquisition by Deaf Children --- p.3 / Chapter 1.3. --- Berent’s (2004) Characterization of Bi-modal Bilingual Acquisition --- p.5 / Chapter 1.4. --- The Current Research --- p.7 / Chapter 1.5. --- Organization of the Thesis --- p.10 / Chapter Chapter 2 --- Deafness and Language Acquisition --- p.12 / Chapter 2.1. --- Introduction --- p.12 / Chapter 2.2. --- General Descriptions about the Deaf Situation in HK --- p.12 / Chapter 2.3. --- The Effects of Deafness on Language Acquisition --- p.13 / Chapter 2.4. --- Factors Affecting Language Acquisition of Deaf Children --- p.14 / Chapter 2.5. --- Acquisition of Syntax by Deaf Children: Theoretical Assumptions . --- p.14 / Chapter 2.5.1. --- Acquisition Studies on Syntactic Development of Deaf Children . --- p.15 / Chapter 2.5.2. --- Interim Discussion --- p.17 / Chapter 2.6. --- Problematic Domains in Syntactic Acquisition --- p.18 / Chapter 2.6.1. --- Problems in the I-system --- p.20 / Chapter 2.6.2. --- Problems in the C-system --- p.20 / Chapter 2.6.3. --- Problems in the D-system --- p.21 / Chapter 2.6.4. --- Interim Summary --- p.22 / Chapter 2.6.5. --- Problems in Acquiring Syntactic Movement --- p.23 / Chapter 2.6.6. --- Problems with Interpreting Empty Categories --- p.30 / Chapter 2.7. --- Summary --- p.31 / Chapter Chapter 3 --- Morpho-syntactic Analysis of Passive bei2 Construction and the Polysemous Forms of bei2 --- p.33 / Chapter 3.1. --- Introduction --- p.33 / Chapter 3.2. --- Cantonese Passives --- p.33 / Chapter 3.2.1. --- Types of Passives in Cantonese --- p.35 / Chapter 3.2.2. --- Grammatical Relations in Cantonese bei2 Passives --- p.38 / Chapter 3.2.3. --- Morpho-syntactic Analysis of Cantonese bei2 Construction --- p.39 / Chapter 3.2.4. --- The Movement Approach --- p.40 / Chapter 3.2.5. --- A Complementation Approach to Cantonese Passives --- p.44 / Chapter 3.2.6. --- Telicity and Delimiting Elements of the Embedded Predicate --- p.51 / Chapter 3.2.7. --- Interim Discussion --- p.55 / Chapter 3.3. --- Polyesmous Forms of bei2 ‘give’ in Cantonese --- p.56 / Chapter 3.4. --- Transfer bei2 in Cantonese --- p.57 / Chapter 3.4.1. --- The Inverted Double Object Construction (IDOC) in Cantonese --- p.58 / Chapter 3.4.2. --- Derivation of the Transfer bei2 Construction --- p.59 / Chapter 3.4.3. --- Two Non-Canonical Transfer bei2 Construction in Cantonese --- p.60 / Chapter 3.4.4. --- Animacy Effects --- p.63 / Chapter 3.5. --- Permissive bei2 Construction in Cantonese --- p.64 / Chapter 3.5.1. --- Permissive bei2 as Object Control Sentences --- p.65 / Chapter 3.5.2. --- Selection of Non-finite Embedded Clause --- p.70 / Chapter 3.6. --- Ambiguity between Permissive and Passive bei2 --- p.71 / Chapter 3.7. --- Summary --- p.73 / Chapter Chapter4 --- Acquiring Bei2 in Cantonese: A Review of Previous Studies . --- p.75 / Chapter 4.1. --- Introduction --- p.75 / Chapter 4.2. --- Acquisition of Cantonese bei2 Passives --- p.75 / Chapter 4.3. --- Acquisition of Passives in Mandarin --- p.77 / Chapter 4.4. --- Issues in the Acquisition of Passives --- p.79 / Chapter 4.4.1. --- Age of Acquisition --- p.79 / Chapter 4.4.2. --- Passives with Actional vs Non-actional Verbs --- p.81 / Chapter 4.4.3. --- Lexical-semantics of the Verb Phrase of Passives --- p.83 / Chapter 4.5. --- Acquisition of Cantonese bei2 Passives by Deaf Children --- p.84 / Chapter 4.6. --- Acquisition of Passives by Deaf Children in Other Languages --- p.87 / Chapter 4.7. --- Acquisition of Other Polysemous Forms --- p.88 / Chapter 4.7.1. --- Acquisition of Permissive bei2 --- p.88 / Chapter 4.7.2. --- Acquisition of Control --- p.88 / Chapter 4.7.3. --- Acquisition of Transfer bei2 --- p.90 / Chapter 4.8. --- Acquisition Predictions --- p.91 / Chapter 4.8.1. --- How do Deaf Children Acquire Passive bei2 in Cantonese? --- p.91 / Chapter 4.8.2. --- What is the Sequence of Acquisition among the three Polysemous bei2? . --- p.91 / Chapter 4.8.3. --- Do Deaf Children Follow a Different Pathway from Hearing Children? --- p.92 / Chapter Chapter 5 --- Methodology --- p.93 / Chapter 5.1. --- Background --- p.93 / Chapter 5.2. --- Subjects --- p.94 / Chapter 5.2.1. --- Deaf Subjects --- p.94 / Chapter 5.2.2. --- Hearing Subjects --- p.101 / Chapter 5.3. --- Experimental Design --- p.102 / Chapter 5.4. --- Picture Selection (PS) - A Comprehension Task --- p.103 / Chapter 5.4.1. --- Design of Test Items --- p.103 / Chapter 5.4.2. --- Verbs Used in the Test Items --- p.104 / Chapter 5.4.3. --- Items for Transfer bei2 --- p.105 / Chapter 5.4.4. --- Items for Permissive bei2 --- p.106 / Chapter 5.4.5. --- Items for Passive bei2 --- p.108 / Chapter 5.4.6. --- Items for Zoeng1 and Active Sentences --- p.108 / Chapter 5.4.7. --- Experimental Procedure --- p.110 / Chapter 5.5. --- Picture Description (PD) - An Elicited Production Task --- p.111 / Chapter 5.5.1. --- Design of Test Items --- p.112 / Chapter 5.5.2. --- Items for Transfer bei2 --- p.113 / Chapter 5.5.3. --- Items for Permissive bei2 --- p.115 / Chapter 5.5.4. --- Items for Passive bei2 --- p.117 / Chapter 5.5.5. --- Active Sentences --- p.120 / Chapter 5.5.6. --- Testing Procedure --- p.121 / Chapter 5.6. --- Picture Verification (PV) - A Judgment Task --- p.123 / Chapter 5.6.1. --- Can1 as the Disambiguating Cue --- p.124 / Chapter 5.6.2. --- Experimental Design --- p.126 / Chapter 5.6.3. --- The Test Items --- p.129 / Chapter 5.7. --- Experimental Procedures --- p.132 / Chapter Chapter 6 --- Results --- p.133 / Chapter 6.1. --- Introduction --- p.133 / Chapter 6.2. --- Performance in Passive bei2 --- p.136 / Chapter 6.2.1. --- Picture Selection (PS) Task --- p.136 / Chapter 6.2.2. --- Picture Description (PD) Task --- p.139 / Chapter 6.2.3. --- Active Sentences (Controls) --- p.139 / Chapter 6.2.4. --- Passive bei2 with an Embedded Bare Verb --- p.141 / Chapter 6.2.5. --- Resultative Passives --- p.148 / Chapter 6.2.6. --- Subjects’ Performance on the Embedded VP --- p.152 / Chapter 6.2.7. --- Reversibility of the NPs --- p.157 / Chapter 6.2.8. --- The Picture Verification (PV) Task --- p.161 / Chapter 6.2.9. --- Is -can1 an Effective Disambiguating Cue? --- p.161 / Chapter 6.2.10. --- Interim Summary --- p.170 / Chapter 6.3. --- Performance on Transfer bei2 and Permissive bei2 --- p.171 / Chapter 6.3.1. --- Picture Selection (PS) Task --- p.171 / Chapter 6.3.2. --- Picture Description (PD) Task --- p.176 / Chapter 6.3.3. --- Acquisition of Transfer bei2 --- p.176 / Chapter 6.3.4. --- Acquisition of Permissive bei2 --- p.183 / Chapter 6.3.5. --- The Three Polysemous Forms in the PD Task --- p.191 / Chapter 6.3.6. --- The Three Polysemous Forms in Both the PS and PD Tasks --- p.193 / Chapter 6.3.7. --- Performance by Level/Group in Both the PS and PD Tasks --- p.196 / Chapter Chapter 7 --- General Discussion and Conclusion --- p.199 / Chapter 7.1. --- Introduction --- p.199 / Chapter 7.2. --- The Acquisition of Passive bei2 Construction in Cantonese --- p.199 / Chapter 7.2.1. --- Knowledge about the Grammatical Relations in Passive bei2 --- p.200 / Chapter 7.2.2. --- Knowledge about the Syntactic Order of Passive bei2 --- p.202 / Chapter 7.2.3. --- Knowledge about the Reversible and Non-Reversible Passives --- p.203 / Chapter 7.2.4. --- Knowledge about a +Telic Embedded Predicate --- p.204 / Chapter 7.2.5. --- Knowledge about Co-indexation and Null Operator Movement . --- p.206 / Chapter 7.3. --- Acquisition of Polysemous Forms of bei2 --- p.207 / Chapter 7.3.1. --- Order of Difficulty --- p.207 / Chapter 7.3.2. --- Acquisition of Transfer bei2 --- p.209 / Chapter 7.3.3. --- Acquisition of Permissive bei2 --- p.211 / Chapter 7.3.4. --- Developmental Pathway of Deaf Children’s Grammatical Knowledge --- p.213 / Chapter 7.4. --- Limitations of the Research --- p.214 / Chapter 7.5. --- Conclusions --- p.217 / Chapter 7.5.1. --- Get to Know the Acquisition Process of Passive bei2s and Other Polysemous Forms --- p.217 / Chapter 7.5.2. --- Age Factor in Research on Language Development of Deaf Children . --- p.218 / Chapter 7.5.3. --- Understanding Language Development of Deaf Children --- p.220 / Chapter 7.5.4. --- Practical Implications --- p.223 / Chapter Appendix 1: --- Test items of the experiment --- p.225 / Chapter Appendix 2: --- Correct Responses in the Picture Selection Task (All hearing adult subjects, N=20) --- p.231 / References --- p.232
288

"Do you know what I think?" a cross-linguistic investigation of children's understanding of mental state words /

Souza, Debora Hollanda, Echols, Catharine H. January 2004 (has links) (PDF)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Texas at Austin, 2004. / Supervisor: Catharine H. Echols. Vita. Includes bibliographical references. Also available from UMI.
289

A pilot study of the relationship between the English language abilities of a group of primary school children and their tree-drawings

Kierman, Wynsome Doreen January 1991 (has links)
This pilot study attempts to assess the correlation between language ability and a projective Tree-drawing test. The hypothesis tested is that expounded by Sandra Michel, a psychologist working with Dr. A. A. Tomatis at the Language Centre in Paris. The theory of Language from which her work derives is that put forward by A. A. Tomatis in Vers l'écoute humaine (Towards Human Hearing), (1977) and Education and Dyslexia trans . , Louise Guiney, (1972). This hypothesis states that the Tree-drawing can give a clear indication of the kind of dialogue the testee will be able and willing to use. The claims for projective Tree-drawing 'tests' or techniques have been mainly concerned with personality or psychological assessments and sometimes with intelligence testing since Charles Koch first began his work in this field in the early forties. Sandra Michel in "The Tree Test", translated by T . Brown, (unpublished paper), Tomatis Centre (Scarbrough, Ontario, 1980) discusses a Tree-drawing scale that indicates both the developmental level of the child's language ability and his/her motivation towards dialogue and communication at this level. To test this hypothesis a sample of 1094 Tree-drawings was used. The drawings were done by Primary School children of both sexes from Sub. A. to Std . 5, collected over four years of research. These drawings were studied to see if the Tree scale of drawings described by Michel did in fact occur and if they occurred in the sequence she suggests. As a result of these preliminary investigations a developmental scale was devised and proposed as a refinement of Michel's scale. Scores from these two Tree-scales were correlated with English language scores using Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficient and the Spearman Rank techniques. The sample for this correlation study was a group of 54 Std. 5 pupils whose English language ability was evaluated by (i) a school English Examination mark, (ii) a Questionnaire scoring receptive and expressive language behaviours and (iii) Verbal I.Q. scores. The positive correlation between these language scores and the Tree-drawing projective test scores are discussed and the implications for English language teaching and suggestions for further research mentioned.
290

Selected Code-Related and Language- Related Factors of Reading Comprehension for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students

Falk, Jodi Lara January 2016 (has links)
This study examined the interrelationships among English language structures (phonological knowledge, morphological knowledge, silent word reading fluency) and reading comprehension in a group of 45 deaf and hard of hearing students in grades 3 to 8, taking into account their demographic characteristics. Simple Pearson correlations, multiple regression analyses, and an independent samples t test were used in this study. Results indicated that morphological knowledge was the significant variable positively associated with reading comprehension over and beyond the other predictors (phonological knowledge and silent word reading fluency) and the demographic covariates (home language, age, and gender).

Page generated in 0.04 seconds