Spelling suggestions: "subject:"cognition"" "subject:"cognitition""
591 |
The impact of Odyssey of the mind on the cognitive and psychosocial development of adolescents /Weeks, Denise M. January 2003 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--Central Connecticut State University, 2003. / Thesis advisor: Jane Fried. " ... in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Student Development in Higher Education." Includes bibliographical references (leaves 82-83). Also available via the World Wide Web.
|
592 |
The effects of cognitive teaching techniques on ninth grade mathematics achievement shifting the balance for special populations /Breeding, Cynthia Ann. January 2002 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Texas at Austin, 2002. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references. Available also from UMI Company.
|
593 |
Dis/embodied choreographyParkins, Michelle Elena 28 June 2012 (has links)
This thesis investigates the intersection of physical and non-physical choreographic practices, culminating in a performed solo dance work titled Lilith. In a world increasingly consumed by virtual technology, the investigation is increasingly relevant to explore both in our personal lives and in the field of dance and choreography. This thesis examines the relationship between embodied and disembodied experiences while exploring the affect of these experiences on the mind cognitively and emotionally. In this thesis I investigate performances and written work about other choreographers’ investigations into computer-mediated methods of disembodying dance, laying the foundation for my own solo performance. Through experiential research into computer mediated-methods of altering choreography, I have explored the effect of non-physicalized ways of generating choreographic movement. I have equally investigated how a physically impulse driven movement has influenced the choreographic process. In the end, this work explores the tensional forces that lie between the physical and non-physical in a creative, choreographic process and attempts to find ways to create balance between the two. / text
|
594 |
Dietary flavonoids and cognitive impairment in Chinese older adults: a cross-sectional studyLi, Wenjia., 李汶嘉. January 2011 (has links)
Introduction:
Cognitive impairment (CI) in older adults refers to impairment in cognition function
beyond normal aging. Previous studies have reported the effect of dietary antioxidants,
especially flavonoids on cognitive function in older adults, suggesting that high
dietary flavonoids intake may reduce the risk of cognitive impairment. However, most
of these studies were reported in Caucasian older populations in Western countries.
Moreover, some studies were positive, which others were negative studies. There was
no previous study regarding dietary flavonoids and cognitive impairment among
Chinese older adults.
Objectives:
The objectives of this study were to explore the association between dietary
intake flavonoids and the risk of cognitive impairment in Chinese older adults, and to
identify the independent factors associated with the risk of cognitive impairment in
Chinese older adults.
Method:
This study was a retrospective cross-sectional study, 200 patients who fulfilled the
inclusion and exclusion criteria and gave written informed consent were recruited
from the Geriatric clinics in Queen Mary Hospital, Hong Kong, from 11th July to 31st
October 2011. All recruited subjects were invited to attend face-to-face interviews.
Brief cognitive assessments by the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) and
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) were done for all subjects. Every subject
also answered a detailed questionnaire on socio-demographic, type and frequency of
diet items (food frequency questionnaire) and co-morbid diseases.
Main outcome measure:
The primary outcome measure was cognitive function, using an education-adjusted
MMSE cut-off s to categorize the subjects into two groups (i.e. normal cognitive
group and cognitively impaired group).
Results:
200 subjects were recruited, 104 participants (37 males and 67 females) were in the
normal cognitive group and 96 subjects (22 males and 74 females) were in the
cognitively impaired group. The mean amount of dietary flavonoids intake of the
cognitively normal group was higher than that of the cognitively impaired group (i.e.
291.84 grams/day and 240.92 grams/day respectively; p=0.006 Mann-Whitney U test).
Other common dietary nutrients were also compared between the two groups. The
dietary carbohydrate (p<0.001), total sugar (p<0.001), vitamin C (p=0.001) and folic
acid (p<0.001) intakes were significantly related to cognitive impairment (using
chi-square statistics).
After adjustment for age, gender, education, exercise pattern, the dietary flavonoids
intake was not independently associated with cognitive impairment. However, after
adjustment for age, education level, exercise pattern and BMI, the higher to highest
quartile of dietary carbohydrate intake (184.184-1176.703g/d), high total sugar intake
(0.166-35.102g/d), highest quartile of vitamin C intake (139.803-900.952mg/d), the
third and fourth quartiles of folic acid intake (208.899-1544.418mcg/d) were
significantly associated with reduced risks of cognitive impairment. Older adults who
had regular exercise (both less and more than 30 minutes per day) had lower risk of
cognitive impairment than those who never did exercise (OR= 0.361, 95% confidence
interval=0.167-0.784 for less than 30 minutes/d; OR=0.137, 95% confidence
interval=0.048-0.396 for more than 30 minutes/d). We also found high BMI was
associated with a reduced risk of cognitive impairment with an OR of 0.897 (95%
confidence interval=0.816-0.987). Besides, the increasing age was also associated
with cognitive impairment (OR=1.059, 95% confidence interval=1.007-1.115).
Conclusion:
In summary, the present study demonstrated that the dietary flavonoids intake
was not significantly associated with the risk of cognitive impairment in Chinese
older adults in Hong Kong. / published_or_final_version / Medicine / Master / Master of Medical Sciences
|
595 |
Effects of cognitive training on enhancing verbal and visual-spatial working memory in older adults at risk of cognitive declineLeung, Tsz-yan, 梁芷欣 January 2014 (has links)
abstract / Psychology / Master / Master of Philosophy
|
596 |
The relationship between executive functions and broad written language skills in students ages 12 to 14 years oldHargrave, Jennifer Leann 28 August 2008 (has links)
Not available / text
|
597 |
THE EFFECTS OF CONTEXT ON MULTIDIMENSIONAL SPATIAL COGNITIVE MODELSDupnick, Edwin Gene January 1979 (has links)
No description available.
|
598 |
The part played by symbols in thinking with special reference to belief and cognate statesEvans, J. L. January 1945 (has links)
No description available.
|
599 |
Relationships in Aging, Cognitive Processes, and Contingency LearningReeder, Sarah 01 August 2006 (has links)
This study investigated the influence of age, processing speed, working memory,and associative processes on the acquisition of contingency information. Young and older adults completed positive (+.65) and negative (-.65) contingency tasks that measured their ability to discover the relationship between a symptom (e.g., FEVER) and a fictional disease (e.g., OLYALGIA). Both d' scores, i.e., contingency learning, and contingency estimates, i.e., contingency judgment, were examined. Participants were also asked to complete cognitive tasks that measure the constructs of processing speed, working memory resources, associative memory, and associative learning. Structural equation modeling was used to examine the direct and indirect relationships between processing speed, working memory resources, associative memory, associative learning, and positive and negative contingency learning and judgment for young and older adult groups. Young adults outperformed older adults on the cognitive tasks and on contingency learning and judgment tasks. However, age differences were smaller for the positive contingency than for negative contingency. A comparison of the structural equation models for young and older adults showed no relationship between any cognitive construct and negative contingency learning. However, young adults' judgment for the negative contingency was directly influenced by associative learning, while their learning and judgment for the positive contingency was directly influenced by associative memory. For older adults, working memory executive function directly influenced their judgment for the negative contingency and their learning and judgment for the positive contingency. Processing speed had an indirect effect on older adults' contingency learning and judgment that was mediated by working memory executive functioning. The differences in the young adults' models as well as the difference between the young and older adults' models for positive and negative contingencies suggest that while associative processing is important, it may not account for all of the variation in contingency learning and judgment. The young adults' models for the negative contingency task indicates that higher level processes, such as inductive reasoning, maybe involved in negative contingency judgment because the associative learning task required some level of hypothesis testing. In contrast, positive contingency learning and judgment could rely primarily on more basic associative processes. The present findings therefore suggest that an overall model of contingency learning must include both associative processes and inductive reasoning processes. Older adults' general contingency performance was most directly related to their working memory executive functioning, suggesting that the decline in their working memory has the strongest effect on their ability to acquire and use information about contingencies. In fact, the age related decline in working memory seems to affect older adults' ability to acquire both positive and negative contingencies. The similarities across the older adult models for positive and negative contingencies indicate that the underlying deficit in older adults' working memory executive functioning that affects their overall contingency learning and judgment performance. This basic working memory executive functioning deficit for older adults also explains why their models for positive and negative contingency did not exhibit direct relationships between associative tasks and contingency learning as observed for the young adult models.
|
600 |
Metacognitive skills and executive functions : an examination of relationships and development in young childrenBryce, Donna Lynne January 2011 (has links)
No description available.
|
Page generated in 0.0763 seconds