Spelling suggestions: "subject:"creative thinking."" "subject:"kreative thinking.""
51 |
Creative problem solving : the roles of moods and emotionsKristiansen, Glenn January 2014 (has links)
No description available.
|
52 |
Creative processes in young childrenStyles, Irene Mavis January 1971 (has links)
Enthusiasm for developing creativity in the individual has been remarkably widespread - at least in America - over the past twenty-five years or so. The armed services, the arts and sciences, educational institutions, businesses and industries are recognising to a greater and greater extent, the urgent necessity of developing this relatively neglected aspect of people is personalities. Their reasons differ, of course, and usually the welfare of the individual himself is not the main concern. This is perhaps fortunate, as advances made on philanthropic grounds alone have never progressed very rapidly. In business and industry, new ideas are urgently needed for survival - this was especially evident after World War II which was, in the end, really a battle of ideas. The importance of this implication has not decreased with distance in time from that conflagration, indeed, individuals in the armed services are probably the people most deeply involved in and the most concerned with the problem of developing creative thought. "We are in a mortal struggle for the survival of our way of life", writes Guilford. "The need (for developing creativity) is a national crisis" says Anderson*. Intro., p. 1.
|
53 |
Nonabsolute/relativistic (N/R) thinking: a possible unifying commonality underlying models of postformal reasoningYan, Bernice Lai-ting 05 1900 (has links)
This dissertation identified and addressed four of the unresolved issues pertaining
to the proposition that nonabsolute/ relativistic (N/R) thinking is one of the possible
unifying commonalities underlying the selected models of postformal reasoning, namely
Problem Finding, Dialectical Reasoning, Relativistic Operations and Reflective
Judgment.
A total of 254 participants aged 10 to 48 and attending Grade 5 to doctoral studies
were involved. Each participant was administered eight tests in pencil-and-paper format
to measure eight different constructs of thinking. Different specific hypotheses were
evaluated through different statistical approaches.
The four identified issues were addressed as follows:
Firstly, nonabsolute/ relativistic thinking was reconceptualized and operationally
defined as a multidimensional and multilevel construct. Two dimensions were proposed:
the basic form and the epistemic view. Within the basic form dimension, two levels were
proposed: the formal and the postformal forms.
Secondly, a battery of three tests was specifically designed by Arlin and the
author to measure the different dimensions and levels of nonabsolute/ relativistic
thinking.
Thirdly, strong empirical evidence was obtained supporting the general
hypothesis that nonabsolute/ relativistic thinking is a possible unifying commonality
underlying the four selected postformal models. Within the construct of nonabsolute/
relativistic thinking, two dimensions, the basic form and the epistemic view, can be
differentiated as hypothesized.
Fourthly, empirical evidence was also obtained supporting the general hypothesis
that nonabsolute/ relativistic thinking is an instance of both formal and postformal
reasoning. Specifically within the basic form dimension, two qualitatively different
forms, the formal and the postformal, can be differentiated as hypothesized. Findings
also suggested that the development of a nonabsolute epistemic view might play a crucial
role in the development of the postformal form. Therefore, the emergence of the
postformal form can be explained by a paradigm shift from an absolute to a nonabsolute
epistemic view. Performances in the tests of the postformal form and of the epistemic
view in combination were found to be good predictors of performances in the selected
postformal tests.
Significant implications of the findings are that nonabsolute/ relativistic thinking
represents a form of metamorphosis from closed-system to open-system thinking and it
might serve as a potential springboard in the development of higher order thinking. / Education, Faculty of / Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education (ECPS), Department of / Graduate
|
54 |
An historical overview of creativity with implications for educationEllis, Antoinette S. 01 January 1986 (has links)
This thesis traced the development of the concept of creativity from the earliest works in the intellectual history of Western civilization to the late twentieth century. This historical perspective on the concept of creativity served as a backdrop to current views of the concept and as a reference source for recurrent views of the concept and as a reference source for recurrent and essential themes in the progressing debates concerning this issue.
|
55 |
Teaching outside of the box : studying a creative teacherLilly, Frank R. January 2001 (has links)
No description available.
|
56 |
An Investigation of creative thinking among college students /Razik, Taher A. January 1963 (has links)
No description available.
|
57 |
The home environment and the creative thinking ability of children /Ellinger, Bernice D. January 1964 (has links)
No description available.
|
58 |
Creative dramatics : instructional methodologies for the elementary curriculum /Fuller, Rex Maxwell January 1973 (has links)
No description available.
|
59 |
The measurement of certain aspects of critical thinking in a mathematical context /Wozniak, Paul Henry January 1976 (has links)
No description available.
|
60 |
The use of squiggling : a play technique as a diagnostic aid in the assessment of secondary school-age children with Asperger syndromeWakerley, Eleanor January 2009 (has links)
Winnicott’s (1968, 1993) play technique squiggling was piloted as a measure of creative thinking abilities and a potential diagnostic aid in the assessment of Asperger syndrome. The internal consistency and inter-rater reliability of squiggling was found to be acceptable. Mixed results were found between the six subscales in terms of concurrent validity with the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT: Torrance, Bal & Safter, 2008). Squiggling subscales Elaboration and Imagination showed signs of psychometric strength. However, Fluency and Originality require revision. Concurrent validity of the subscales Flexibility and Integration were not established. A matched-participants design enabled testing of hypothesised differences in creative thinking abilities with children with Asperger syndrome using the TTCT (Torrance et al., 2008): Abstractedness, Fluency, Originality, Integration, Elaboration, Resistance to Premature Foreclosure and Flexibility. Children with Asperger syndrome demonstrated a significantly higher level of elaboration and abstract imagination in their drawings relative to a comparison group of typically-developing children matched on age, visual motor integration ability and non-verbal IQ. Findings indicate partial support for the Weak Central Coherence Theory (Shah and Frith, 1983) and Leslie’s (1987) Meta-Representational Deficit hypothesis. Children with Asperger syndrome demonstrated understanding and expression of abstract concepts as graphical representations, thereby supporting their use in clinical assessments and interventions. No support was found for the Executive Dysfunction Theory (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996) or for the Hyper-Systemising Theory (Baron-Cohen, 2006). Some limitations include the heterogeneity of the clinical group, and the possible confounding effects of verbal intellectual abilities, extrinsic rewards and performance anxiety. Major strengths of the study include a successful matching procedure and the finding of group differences with large effect sizes on particular creative thinking abilities.
|
Page generated in 0.0701 seconds