• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • Tagged with
  • 5
  • 5
  • 3
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Cognitive behavioural models of chronic pain and the role of selective attention

Dehghani, Mohsen January 2003 (has links)
Cognitive-behavioural based models of chronic pain contend that appraisals of harm affect the individual�s response to pain. It has been suggested that fear of pain and/or anxiety sensitivity predispose individuals to chronicity. However, other factors such as pain self-efficacy are believed to mediate between experience of pain and disability. According to this view, pain is maintained through hypervigilance towards painful sensations and subsequent avoidance. Four studies were conducted in order to evaluate the structure of fear-avoidance models of chronic pain, and also, to examine the role of hypervigilance as an underlying mechanism in maintenance of pain. In study one, using a sample of 207 consecutive patients, two models were tested. First, fear of movement model as proposed by Vlaeyen et al. (1995a) was examined. It was found that negative affectivity has direct effects on the fear and avoidance of pain, which in turn, contributes to disability. In total, fear/avoidance accounted for a significant amount of the variance of disability. In addition, severity of pain was found to increase pain disability, while itself is influenced still by negative affectivity. These findings supported the model of fear of pain as described by Vlaeyen et al. (1995a). Further, we found that self-efficacy may mediate the impact of fear of pain on disability and reduces the perceived physical disability. At the same time, self-efficacy was shown to have direct reductive impact on disability. However, both studies indicated that people who are fearful in response to pain are more likely to develop disability, although self-efficacy may play a moderating role. In the studies one, two, and three, the role of hypervigilance in over attending to pain was investigated. In study one a large sample of 168 chronic pain patients were studied. Questionnaires measuring different aspects of pain and a computerised version of the Dot-Probe Task were administered. Four types of words related to different dimensions of pain and matched neutral words were used as stimuli. Reaction times in response to the stimuli were recorded. A factorial design 3x4x2x2 and ANOVAs were employed to analyse the data. Chronic pain patients showed a cognitive bias to sensory pain words relative to affective, disability, and threat-related words. However, contrary to expectations, those high in fear of pain responded more slowly to stimuli than those less fearful of pain. These results suggest that patients with chronic pain problems selectively attend to sensory aspects of pain. However, selective attention appears to depend upon the nature of pain stimuli. For those who are highly fearful of pain they may not only selectively attend to pain-related information but also have difficulty disengaging from those stimuli. In study two, 35 chronic pain patients were compared with the same number matched healthy subjects. Both groups completed measures of fear of pain, anxiety sensitivity, depression and anxiety, in addition to dot probe task. Results indicated that both groups show similar attentional bias to sensory words in comparison with other word types. However, the level of this biasness was higher for chronic pain patients. Lack of significant differences between patients and controls is discussed in the context of possible evolutionary value of sensitivity to pain as an adaptive reaction in healthy controls, and contrary, as a maladaptive response to pain in chronic pain patients. The results of the previous research suggest that chronic pain patients demonstrate cognitive biases towards pain-related information and that such biases predict patient functioning. The forth study examined the degree to which a successful cognitive-behavioural program was able to modify the observed attentional bias towards sensory pain words. Forty-two patients with chronic pain conditions for more than three months were recruited prior to commencing a cognitive-behavioural pain management program. Participants were assessed before the program, after the program and at one-month follow-up. Results confirmed that chronic pain patients exhibited biased attention towards sensory pain-related words at pre-treatment. These biases were still evident at post-treatment, but were no longer statistically significant at follow up. Multiple regression analyses indicated that the changes in attentional bias towards sensory words between post-treatment and follow-up were predicted by pre- to post- treatment changes in fear of movement (Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia) but not other relevant variables, such as fear of pain or anxiety sensitivity. These results demonstrate that successful cognitive-behavioural treatments can reduce selective attention, thought to be indicative of hypervigilance towards pain. Moreover, these biases appear to be changed by reducing the fear associated with movement. Theoretically, these results provide support for the fear of (re)injury model of pain. Clinically, this study supports the contention that fear of (re)injury and movement is an appropriate target of pain management and that reducing these fears causes patients to attend less to pain-related stimuli.
2

Cognitive behavioural models of chronic pain and the role of selective attention

Dehghani, Mohsen January 2003 (has links)
Cognitive-behavioural based models of chronic pain contend that appraisals of harm affect the individual�s response to pain. It has been suggested that fear of pain and/or anxiety sensitivity predispose individuals to chronicity. However, other factors such as pain self-efficacy are believed to mediate between experience of pain and disability. According to this view, pain is maintained through hypervigilance towards painful sensations and subsequent avoidance. Four studies were conducted in order to evaluate the structure of fear-avoidance models of chronic pain, and also, to examine the role of hypervigilance as an underlying mechanism in maintenance of pain. In study one, using a sample of 207 consecutive patients, two models were tested. First, fear of movement model as proposed by Vlaeyen et al. (1995a) was examined. It was found that negative affectivity has direct effects on the fear and avoidance of pain, which in turn, contributes to disability. In total, fear/avoidance accounted for a significant amount of the variance of disability. In addition, severity of pain was found to increase pain disability, while itself is influenced still by negative affectivity. These findings supported the model of fear of pain as described by Vlaeyen et al. (1995a). Further, we found that self-efficacy may mediate the impact of fear of pain on disability and reduces the perceived physical disability. At the same time, self-efficacy was shown to have direct reductive impact on disability. However, both studies indicated that people who are fearful in response to pain are more likely to develop disability, although self-efficacy may play a moderating role. In the studies one, two, and three, the role of hypervigilance in over attending to pain was investigated. In study one a large sample of 168 chronic pain patients were studied. Questionnaires measuring different aspects of pain and a computerised version of the Dot-Probe Task were administered. Four types of words related to different dimensions of pain and matched neutral words were used as stimuli. Reaction times in response to the stimuli were recorded. A factorial design 3x4x2x2 and ANOVAs were employed to analyse the data. Chronic pain patients showed a cognitive bias to sensory pain words relative to affective, disability, and threat-related words. However, contrary to expectations, those high in fear of pain responded more slowly to stimuli than those less fearful of pain. These results suggest that patients with chronic pain problems selectively attend to sensory aspects of pain. However, selective attention appears to depend upon the nature of pain stimuli. For those who are highly fearful of pain they may not only selectively attend to pain-related information but also have difficulty disengaging from those stimuli. In study two, 35 chronic pain patients were compared with the same number matched healthy subjects. Both groups completed measures of fear of pain, anxiety sensitivity, depression and anxiety, in addition to dot probe task. Results indicated that both groups show similar attentional bias to sensory words in comparison with other word types. However, the level of this biasness was higher for chronic pain patients. Lack of significant differences between patients and controls is discussed in the context of possible evolutionary value of sensitivity to pain as an adaptive reaction in healthy controls, and contrary, as a maladaptive response to pain in chronic pain patients. The results of the previous research suggest that chronic pain patients demonstrate cognitive biases towards pain-related information and that such biases predict patient functioning. The forth study examined the degree to which a successful cognitive-behavioural program was able to modify the observed attentional bias towards sensory pain words. Forty-two patients with chronic pain conditions for more than three months were recruited prior to commencing a cognitive-behavioural pain management program. Participants were assessed before the program, after the program and at one-month follow-up. Results confirmed that chronic pain patients exhibited biased attention towards sensory pain-related words at pre-treatment. These biases were still evident at post-treatment, but were no longer statistically significant at follow up. Multiple regression analyses indicated that the changes in attentional bias towards sensory words between post-treatment and follow-up were predicted by pre- to post- treatment changes in fear of movement (Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia) but not other relevant variables, such as fear of pain or anxiety sensitivity. These results demonstrate that successful cognitive-behavioural treatments can reduce selective attention, thought to be indicative of hypervigilance towards pain. Moreover, these biases appear to be changed by reducing the fear associated with movement. Theoretically, these results provide support for the fear of (re)injury model of pain. Clinically, this study supports the contention that fear of (re)injury and movement is an appropriate target of pain management and that reducing these fears causes patients to attend less to pain-related stimuli.
3

Psychological and Genetic Predictors of Pain Sensitivity

Li, May, Walsh, Keith, Patanwala, Sid, Snyder, Eric January 2013 (has links)
Class of 2013 Abstract / Specific Aims: To assess influence of PCS and FPQ-III on pain tolerance as well as SNPs TRPA1(rs11988795), COMT (rs4646312, rs6269) and FAAH(rs 932816, rs4141964, rs2295633). Methods: A Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) and Fear of Pain Questionnaire (FPQ-III) were completed by a total of 89 healthy adults. A genetic analysis from cheek swabs was performed for single nucleotide polymorphisms(SNPs) within genes: TRPA1, COMT, and FAAH. A cold-pressor test involving the non-dominant hand inserted in circulating water kept at 1-3 degrees Celsius was used and the duration of time subjects were able to leave their hand in the water (pain tolerance) was measured as the primary outcome. Linear regression analysis was used to identify predictors of pain tolerance. Main Results: The subjects were 58% female, the majority were Caucasian (51%) with 26% Asian, 14% Hispanic and 9% other. The mean pain tolerance was 121 ± 66 seconds and regression analysis showed female sex (p=0.001), Asian race (p=0.001), PCS score (<0.001) and FPQ-III score (p=0.014) were associated with decreased pain tolerance while the SNPs were not.      Conclusion: Psychological factors and patient demographics are associated with pain tolerance but the single nucleotide polymorphisms evaluated were not. Future pain studies should utilize a psychological assessment to adjust for this as a confounder.
4

The association between pain-related functioning and psychological disorders in pediatric racial/ethnic minorities with chronic pain

Srinath, Aarabhi Namrata 09 March 2024 (has links)
Pain catastrophizing (PC) and fear of pain (FOP) are understudied across different race/ethnicity minorities. The association between these constructs and psychological disorders with relation to chronic pain are understudied as well. Prior research indicates that racial/ethnic minority populations may engage in more PC and FOP than white, non-Hispanic populations. These studies, however, have only examined macro-level differences between white, non-Hispanic and minority populations. Less is known about the nuanced differences in PC and FOP across individual racial/ethnic groups (i.e., white vs. Asian vs. Black/African American). The current study explores between-group differences in PC and FOP across diverse racial/ethnic groups of youth with chronic pain while also observing the association between anxiety/depression and PC/FOP in these populations. Youth (ages 11-17) with chronic pain presenting for treatment to a tertiary pediatric pain clinic completed the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS), which includes a total score and subscales (i.e., rumination, magnification, and helplessness) and the Fear of Pain Questionnaire. Racial/ethnic group sizes were as follows: Black/non-Hispanic (N = 29), Hispanic (N = 58), Asian (N = 17), another race/non-Hispanic (N = 37), and Multiracial (N = 15). One-way ANOVAs were conducted to test differences in the PCS total score and subscales as well as FOP among racial/ethnic groups, and Chi-square analyses were conducted to test the association between binary codes of anxiety/depression diagnoses provided in the data repository and the race/ethnicity minorities, respectively. Results revealed non-significant differences in total PCS and PCS subscales across race/ethnicity minority groups. In addition, there were no statistically significant differences in FOP across race/ethnicity minority groups. However, there were some clinically significant differences between mean PCS and FOP scores across certain racial/ethnic minority groups. Finally, no significant associations emerged between anxiety and race/ethnicities or between depression and race/ethnicities. Findings suggest that youth with chronic pain may experience PC and FOP similarly regardless of their racial/ethnic backgrounds. However, these findings were limited by small sample sizes across groups, and future research with larger sample sizes is warranted. These results can inform continued exploration and sensitivity to diversity, equity, and inclusivity issues in healthcare for pediatric chronic pain patients.
5

The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on the Experience and Spreading of Pain

Holmström, Claudia, Ryderås, Cecilia January 2018 (has links)
No description available.

Page generated in 0.0915 seconds