• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 58
  • 9
  • 7
  • 6
  • 2
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 95
  • 43
  • 17
  • 16
  • 11
  • 10
  • 9
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 7
  • 7
  • 7
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
71

"Die vrugbare tuin van Fallus" 'n Beliggaamde beeld van die manlike psige onder mediese pasiënte (Afrikaans)

Pienaar, Shane Bridget 03 October 2005 (has links)
Please read the abstract in the section 00front of this document / Dissertation (DPhil (Psychology))--University of Pretoria, 2005. / Psychology / unrestricted
72

Refining the understatement penalty in terms of the Tax Administration Act / Johannes Alexander Feuth

Feuth, Johannes Alexander January 2013 (has links)
The Tax Administration Act (28 of 2011) (TA Act), which was promulgated on 4 July 2012 and came into effect on 1 October 2012, was enacted with the purpose of aligning all the administrative provisions dealt with under the various sections of the Income Tax Act (58 of 1962) (IT Act) and the Value-Added Tax Act (89 of 1991) (VAT Act) under one piece of legislation. The TA Act (28 of 2011) provides guidance on various matters of tax administration, including a very controversial penalty levying regime. Prior to the TA Act (28 of 2011), section 76 of the IT Act (58 of 1962) and section 60 the VAT Act (89 of 1991) (hereafter referred to as the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions) dealt with the levying of additional taxes in cases of understated tax returns. Sections 76 and 60 of the respective acts unfortunately did not provide proper guidelines on the assessment and calculation of these additional taxes or on how the levying of these additional taxes could conform to matters of administrative justice. These matters have been included under sections 221 to 223 of the TA Act (28 of 2011) (hereafter referred to as the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011)) and have been welcomed by most taxpayers. This research study focused on the critical evaluation of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) as well as the provisions which were repealed and replaced by the TA Act (28 of 2011) and which were previously applied in terms of the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. A comparison between the latter provisions, the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) and foreign legislation is made with the purpose of addressing how effective and fair the TA Act (28 of 2011) will prove to be. The study also includes brief advice on any possible improvements or practical approaches regarding the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011). It is also seen as necessary to evaluate the effectiveness of the regulations promulgated in terms of sections 221 to 223 of the TA Act (28 of 2011), and to identify possible problems with the application and interpretation of the relevant understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) by the Commissioner. A literature review was used to critically analyse and compare various pieces of legislation and precedents, including South African and foreign laws and legislation, with possible practical illustrative examples. The objective with the literature review was to clarify issues such as the fairness of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) and the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. The findings of the research study revealed that the enactment of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) on 1 October 2012 partially achieves the objective of providing taxpayers with a penalty levying system that is more reasonable and fair in comparison with the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. Despite a more favourable outcome achieved by the TA Act (28 of 2011), the research concludes that proper guidance and measures for levying a penalty are still lacking and that the legislation is unfortunately still failing in this regard. Harsh penalty percentages based on certain behavioural criteria that are not defined create the need for obvious improvements. That said, the TA Act (28 of 2011) is still young and creates a basis on which further amendments and improvements can take place. / MCom (South African and International Taxation), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2014
73

The long-term effects of fluoxetine on stress-related behaviour and acute monoaminergic stress response in stress sensitive rats / Nico Johan Badenhorst

Badenhorst, Nico Johan January 2014 (has links)
Fluoxetine and escitalopram are the only antidepressants approved by the Food and Drug Administration of the United States of America (FDA) for treatment of major depression in children and adolescents. Both drugs are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). In recent years there has been a growing concern over the long-term developmental effects of early-life exposure to SSRIs. The current study employed male Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) rats, a well described and validated translational model of depression, to investigate the long term effects of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure. First we examined the effect of such early-life exposure on the development of depressive-like behaviour, locomotor activity and anxiety-like behaviour as manifested in early adulthood. Next, the current study investigated the effect of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure on the acute monoaminergic stress response, as displayed later in life. Animals received either saline (vehicle control), or 10 mg/kg/day fluoxetine from postnatal day (ND+) 21 to ND+34 (pre-puberty). The treatment period was chosen to coincide with a developmental phase where the serotonergic system’s neurodevelopment had been completed, yet the noradrenergic and dopaminergic systems had not, a scenario comparable to neurodevelopment in human adolescents. Both behavioural and in vivo intra-cerebral microdialysis experiments were conducted after ND+60 (early adulthood). On ND+60 rats allocated to behavioural experiments were evaluated for depressive-like behaviour in the forced swim test (FST), locomotor activity in the open field test (OFT), and anxiety-like behaviour in the OFT. Corticosterone concentrations were shown to be significantly higher in male FSL rats exposed to a 10 minute forced swim stress when compared to male FSL rats not exposed to a forced swim stress on ND+60. In the microdialysis experiments the rats were exposed to an acute 10 minute forced swim stress and the concentrations of the monoamines and their metabolites were measured before, during, and after the acute stressor. Relative to saline-treated (control) rats, fluoxetine-treated FSL rats did not show long-term changes in immobility in the FST (i.e. no anti-depressant-like activity) on ND+60. Like-wise anxiety-like behaviour in the OFT did not change. However, a significant decrease in locomotor activity was observed in fluoxetine-treated FSL rats compared to saline-treated (control) rats. These data suggest that a long-lasting anti-depressant-like effect of fluoxetine may be masked by the effect on locomotor activity. With measurements from the microdialysis experiments a significant attenuation of the noradrenergic stress response was observed in fluoxetine-treated rats compared to saline controls. A similar picture was observed for 5-hydroxyindole-3-acetic acid (5-HIAA), a metabolite of serotonin (5-HT), although the latter was not statistically significant. At baseline, before the stressor, significant increase in dopamine (DA) levels were observed in fluoxetine treated rats when compared to saline controls, suggesting that enhanced dopamine neurotransmission may comprise a long-term effect of pre-pubertal fluoxetine treatment. There were no discernible differences in homovanilllic acid (HVA) concentrations between fluoxetine-treated rats and saline controls. In conclusion significant developmental effects of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure were observed later in life and these findings warrant further investigation. / MPharm (Pharmacology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2015
74

The long-term effects of methamphetamine on depressive-like behaviour and neuroplasticity in stress-sensitive rats / Moné Mouton

Mouton, Moné January 2014 (has links)
Methamphetamine (METH) abuse has become a fast growing drug problem that has developed into a global epidemic. In fact, METH is one of the most commonly abused substances with an estimated 35 million abusers worldwide and is said to be the second most popular illicit drug. The Western Province of South Africa has seen a dramatic increase in drug abuse in recent years where METH is the primary or secondary drug of abuse. Interestingly, more than 50% of these individuals are under the age of 20 years. The longer duration of euphoric effects of METH has attracted many users away from cocaine in favour of METH. In addition to the rapid euphoric effect of METH, the direct short-term effects include arousal, reduced fatigue, an increase in blood pressure, reduced appetite as well as sustained attention. Chronic METH abuse may result in debilitating and long-lasting effects that includes mood disorders such as depression. Studies suggest a strong relationship between exposure to adverse environmental factors early in life and the later development of a neuropsychiatric disorder, such as depression. However, these severe consequences do not seem to invoke cessation of the drug. The euphoric and addictive properties of METH causes users to abuse the drug with an increase in frequency and dose, even though it might not have been their original intention. The primary objective of this study was to investigate the effect of early-life administration of METH to stress-sensitive (Flinders Sensitive Line - FSL) and control (Flinders Resistant Line - FRL) rats on depressive-like behaviour and regional brain monoamine levels later in life. The study implemented a sixteen-day period for administration of METH or a vehicle control from postnatal day 19 (PnD19) to postnatal day 34 (PnD34). The latter developmental stage corresponds to pre-adolescence in the rat when neurological development are similar to that seen in human adolescents, and represents the stage when drug abuse is most common in humans. Chronic dosing of METH and saline was performed twice daily at 09:00 and at 15:00. The animals received a sub-cutaneous (SC) escalating dose regimen of METH during the 16 day period (mimicking binging behaviour in humans), with every dose escalating in increments of 0.2 mg/kg from 0.2 mg/kg to 6.0 mg/kg. The study then investigated whether early-life administration of METH would cause depressive-like behaviours directly after the injection period (immediate drug effects before withdrawal on PnD35) or later in life (after the withdrawal period in early adulthood on PnD60). The behavioural effects were assessed in a battery of tests and thereafter the rats were sacrificed and the frontal cortex removed and snap frozen for later analyses of altered neurochemistry. The study demonstrated that chronic METH treatment during pre-adolescence induces significant behavioural changes related to depression in humans directly after the injection period (PnD35) and later in life (PnD60). The animals displayed antidepressant-like behaviour in the forced swim test (FST) before withdrawal, yet a depressogenic effect was observed 25 days post-withdrawal. This effect also seems to be additive to the congenital depressive-like phenotype of FSL rats, suggesting a role for genetic susceptibility. This observation would be in line with the two-hit hypothesis of depression, suggesting that the manifestation of depression will result when a genetic predisposition is followed by an environmental stressor (i.e. METH) later in life. The data suggests a working hypothesis that individuals that already have a predisposition to depression may be more susceptible to developing depression when abusing METH. The fact that the FSL control rats were more immobile than FRL control rats also confirmed the face validity of the FSL genetic rat model of depression. Locomotor activity assessment indicated that METH treatment decreased locomotor activity in FSL and FRL rats compared to their vehicle controls on PnD35 but not on PnD60. It is important to note that the effects observed in locomotor activity could not have contributed to the immobility observed in the FST, confirming that the immobility in the FST indeed reflects psychomotor and not locomotor effects. The study also demonstrated that METH significantly lowers social interaction behaviour in both FRL and FSL rats, both immediately following drug treatment (PnD35) and after withdrawal (PnD60). It is therefore clear that this effect of METH is long-lasting, putatively related to neurodevelopmental effects. In addition, the rats investigated the familiar object for a greater amount of time in the novel object recognition test (nORT) on PnD35 and PnD60 and may be the result of loss of recognition memory for the familiar object. This data confirms that METH results in cognitive memory deficits probably due to sustained adverse neurodevelopmental effects. Neurochemical analyses of the frontal cortex indicated decreased serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE) levels on PnD35. METH is widely recognised for its pro-inflammatory effects, while the reduced 5-HT levels observed may have been the result of an increase in circulating pro-inflammatory cytokines. Neurochemical analyses provided thought-provoking data concerning the role of the permissive hypotheses of depression, indicating that dopamine (DA) is most likely not responsible for the behavioural effects observed, at least under the current study conditions, whereas 5-HT is decidedly more involved than expected. The data also suggest that depletion in NE plays a role in the development of depressive-like behaviours following METH exposure. Based on these findings, we propose that disturbances in 5-HT and NE are a crucial mechanism in how METH abuse may precipitate or worsen depressive-like symptoms in individuals who abuse METH. It should be noted that this study does not discard the role of DA in the development of depression after METH exposure, although under the current study conditions it appears that DA does not play a central role. The current study demonstrated that pre-adolescent exposure to METH can reproduce most of the behavioural changes seen in depressed individuals, and that these behavioural data can be used to identify causal neurochemical factors. Environmental stressors such as METH abuse should be regarded as an additional diagnostic criterion and is relevant to an accumulative risk factor hypothesis. Furthermore, although further study is required, the data suggests that early-life exposure to METH may predispose an individual to mood disorders and behavioural abnormalities later in life. / MSc (Pharmacology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2015
75

An exploration of the coping strategies of early adolescents in two Gauteng schools / Anneté Nel

Nel, Anneté January 2014 (has links)
In the South African context adolescents need to cope with societal and family-related stressors seen as everyday stressors, such as parental divorce, violence in communities, bereavement and pressure in schools. Research has shown that these everyday stressors could have negative effects on their well-being. It was further seen that such a high exposure to everyday stress like school-based stressors leads to an increase in unhealthy behaviour, such as smoking and alcohol use in early adolescents. It is for this reason that resilience in adolescence is such an important factor. With all the challenges and everyday stress experienced by early adolescents, the wellness and resiliency perspective adds a positive angle to adolescent functioning. Constructively coping with everyday stress can therefore lead towards well-being. The purpose of this study was to explore how early adolescents cope with everyday stress. A qualitative research method was used with an embedded case study research design. A voluntary sample was drawn, consisting of 15 South African early adolescents from two schools in Gauteng (ages 12 to 15 years; boys and girls). Data on the subjective experience of coping strategies with everyday stress was gathered through individual semi-structured interviews (ten participants) and drawings to aid the verbalisation of their coping strategies. A focus group discussion with a different set of five participants added to the richness of data and to crystallisation, as they discussed themes which emerged from the individual interviews. Data were analysed thematically and visually. The results indicated that early adolescents experienced different types of everyday stress which accordingly needed a variety of coping strategies. Participants coped using intrapersonal resources (existential belief, creative activities, cognitive coping), interpersonal strengths (family, friends, social media) and behaviour-focussed coping (physical activity, fine motor behaviour). These coping strategies linked with the six modalities outlined in Lahad’s BASIC Ph resiliency model, namely beliefs, affect, social interaction, imagery, cognition and physical activity. Of special interest was the clear indication of the role of social media as part of their coping strategies. / MA (Research Psychology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2014
76

Refining the understatement penalty in terms of the Tax Administration Act / Johannes Alexander Feuth

Feuth, Johannes Alexander January 2013 (has links)
The Tax Administration Act (28 of 2011) (TA Act), which was promulgated on 4 July 2012 and came into effect on 1 October 2012, was enacted with the purpose of aligning all the administrative provisions dealt with under the various sections of the Income Tax Act (58 of 1962) (IT Act) and the Value-Added Tax Act (89 of 1991) (VAT Act) under one piece of legislation. The TA Act (28 of 2011) provides guidance on various matters of tax administration, including a very controversial penalty levying regime. Prior to the TA Act (28 of 2011), section 76 of the IT Act (58 of 1962) and section 60 the VAT Act (89 of 1991) (hereafter referred to as the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions) dealt with the levying of additional taxes in cases of understated tax returns. Sections 76 and 60 of the respective acts unfortunately did not provide proper guidelines on the assessment and calculation of these additional taxes or on how the levying of these additional taxes could conform to matters of administrative justice. These matters have been included under sections 221 to 223 of the TA Act (28 of 2011) (hereafter referred to as the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011)) and have been welcomed by most taxpayers. This research study focused on the critical evaluation of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) as well as the provisions which were repealed and replaced by the TA Act (28 of 2011) and which were previously applied in terms of the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. A comparison between the latter provisions, the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) and foreign legislation is made with the purpose of addressing how effective and fair the TA Act (28 of 2011) will prove to be. The study also includes brief advice on any possible improvements or practical approaches regarding the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011). It is also seen as necessary to evaluate the effectiveness of the regulations promulgated in terms of sections 221 to 223 of the TA Act (28 of 2011), and to identify possible problems with the application and interpretation of the relevant understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) by the Commissioner. A literature review was used to critically analyse and compare various pieces of legislation and precedents, including South African and foreign laws and legislation, with possible practical illustrative examples. The objective with the literature review was to clarify issues such as the fairness of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) and the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. The findings of the research study revealed that the enactment of the understatement penalty percentage provisions under the TA Act (28 of 2011) on 1 October 2012 partially achieves the objective of providing taxpayers with a penalty levying system that is more reasonable and fair in comparison with the pre-TA Act (28 of 2011) penalty provisions. Despite a more favourable outcome achieved by the TA Act (28 of 2011), the research concludes that proper guidance and measures for levying a penalty are still lacking and that the legislation is unfortunately still failing in this regard. Harsh penalty percentages based on certain behavioural criteria that are not defined create the need for obvious improvements. That said, the TA Act (28 of 2011) is still young and creates a basis on which further amendments and improvements can take place. / MCom (South African and International Taxation), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2014
77

The long-term effects of fluoxetine on stress-related behaviour and acute monoaminergic stress response in stress sensitive rats / Nico Johan Badenhorst

Badenhorst, Nico Johan January 2014 (has links)
Fluoxetine and escitalopram are the only antidepressants approved by the Food and Drug Administration of the United States of America (FDA) for treatment of major depression in children and adolescents. Both drugs are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). In recent years there has been a growing concern over the long-term developmental effects of early-life exposure to SSRIs. The current study employed male Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) rats, a well described and validated translational model of depression, to investigate the long term effects of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure. First we examined the effect of such early-life exposure on the development of depressive-like behaviour, locomotor activity and anxiety-like behaviour as manifested in early adulthood. Next, the current study investigated the effect of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure on the acute monoaminergic stress response, as displayed later in life. Animals received either saline (vehicle control), or 10 mg/kg/day fluoxetine from postnatal day (ND+) 21 to ND+34 (pre-puberty). The treatment period was chosen to coincide with a developmental phase where the serotonergic system’s neurodevelopment had been completed, yet the noradrenergic and dopaminergic systems had not, a scenario comparable to neurodevelopment in human adolescents. Both behavioural and in vivo intra-cerebral microdialysis experiments were conducted after ND+60 (early adulthood). On ND+60 rats allocated to behavioural experiments were evaluated for depressive-like behaviour in the forced swim test (FST), locomotor activity in the open field test (OFT), and anxiety-like behaviour in the OFT. Corticosterone concentrations were shown to be significantly higher in male FSL rats exposed to a 10 minute forced swim stress when compared to male FSL rats not exposed to a forced swim stress on ND+60. In the microdialysis experiments the rats were exposed to an acute 10 minute forced swim stress and the concentrations of the monoamines and their metabolites were measured before, during, and after the acute stressor. Relative to saline-treated (control) rats, fluoxetine-treated FSL rats did not show long-term changes in immobility in the FST (i.e. no anti-depressant-like activity) on ND+60. Like-wise anxiety-like behaviour in the OFT did not change. However, a significant decrease in locomotor activity was observed in fluoxetine-treated FSL rats compared to saline-treated (control) rats. These data suggest that a long-lasting anti-depressant-like effect of fluoxetine may be masked by the effect on locomotor activity. With measurements from the microdialysis experiments a significant attenuation of the noradrenergic stress response was observed in fluoxetine-treated rats compared to saline controls. A similar picture was observed for 5-hydroxyindole-3-acetic acid (5-HIAA), a metabolite of serotonin (5-HT), although the latter was not statistically significant. At baseline, before the stressor, significant increase in dopamine (DA) levels were observed in fluoxetine treated rats when compared to saline controls, suggesting that enhanced dopamine neurotransmission may comprise a long-term effect of pre-pubertal fluoxetine treatment. There were no discernible differences in homovanilllic acid (HVA) concentrations between fluoxetine-treated rats and saline controls. In conclusion significant developmental effects of pre-pubertal fluoxetine exposure were observed later in life and these findings warrant further investigation. / MPharm (Pharmacology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2015
78

The long-term effects of methamphetamine on depressive-like behaviour and neuroplasticity in stress-sensitive rats / Moné Mouton

Mouton, Moné January 2014 (has links)
Methamphetamine (METH) abuse has become a fast growing drug problem that has developed into a global epidemic. In fact, METH is one of the most commonly abused substances with an estimated 35 million abusers worldwide and is said to be the second most popular illicit drug. The Western Province of South Africa has seen a dramatic increase in drug abuse in recent years where METH is the primary or secondary drug of abuse. Interestingly, more than 50% of these individuals are under the age of 20 years. The longer duration of euphoric effects of METH has attracted many users away from cocaine in favour of METH. In addition to the rapid euphoric effect of METH, the direct short-term effects include arousal, reduced fatigue, an increase in blood pressure, reduced appetite as well as sustained attention. Chronic METH abuse may result in debilitating and long-lasting effects that includes mood disorders such as depression. Studies suggest a strong relationship between exposure to adverse environmental factors early in life and the later development of a neuropsychiatric disorder, such as depression. However, these severe consequences do not seem to invoke cessation of the drug. The euphoric and addictive properties of METH causes users to abuse the drug with an increase in frequency and dose, even though it might not have been their original intention. The primary objective of this study was to investigate the effect of early-life administration of METH to stress-sensitive (Flinders Sensitive Line - FSL) and control (Flinders Resistant Line - FRL) rats on depressive-like behaviour and regional brain monoamine levels later in life. The study implemented a sixteen-day period for administration of METH or a vehicle control from postnatal day 19 (PnD19) to postnatal day 34 (PnD34). The latter developmental stage corresponds to pre-adolescence in the rat when neurological development are similar to that seen in human adolescents, and represents the stage when drug abuse is most common in humans. Chronic dosing of METH and saline was performed twice daily at 09:00 and at 15:00. The animals received a sub-cutaneous (SC) escalating dose regimen of METH during the 16 day period (mimicking binging behaviour in humans), with every dose escalating in increments of 0.2 mg/kg from 0.2 mg/kg to 6.0 mg/kg. The study then investigated whether early-life administration of METH would cause depressive-like behaviours directly after the injection period (immediate drug effects before withdrawal on PnD35) or later in life (after the withdrawal period in early adulthood on PnD60). The behavioural effects were assessed in a battery of tests and thereafter the rats were sacrificed and the frontal cortex removed and snap frozen for later analyses of altered neurochemistry. The study demonstrated that chronic METH treatment during pre-adolescence induces significant behavioural changes related to depression in humans directly after the injection period (PnD35) and later in life (PnD60). The animals displayed antidepressant-like behaviour in the forced swim test (FST) before withdrawal, yet a depressogenic effect was observed 25 days post-withdrawal. This effect also seems to be additive to the congenital depressive-like phenotype of FSL rats, suggesting a role for genetic susceptibility. This observation would be in line with the two-hit hypothesis of depression, suggesting that the manifestation of depression will result when a genetic predisposition is followed by an environmental stressor (i.e. METH) later in life. The data suggests a working hypothesis that individuals that already have a predisposition to depression may be more susceptible to developing depression when abusing METH. The fact that the FSL control rats were more immobile than FRL control rats also confirmed the face validity of the FSL genetic rat model of depression. Locomotor activity assessment indicated that METH treatment decreased locomotor activity in FSL and FRL rats compared to their vehicle controls on PnD35 but not on PnD60. It is important to note that the effects observed in locomotor activity could not have contributed to the immobility observed in the FST, confirming that the immobility in the FST indeed reflects psychomotor and not locomotor effects. The study also demonstrated that METH significantly lowers social interaction behaviour in both FRL and FSL rats, both immediately following drug treatment (PnD35) and after withdrawal (PnD60). It is therefore clear that this effect of METH is long-lasting, putatively related to neurodevelopmental effects. In addition, the rats investigated the familiar object for a greater amount of time in the novel object recognition test (nORT) on PnD35 and PnD60 and may be the result of loss of recognition memory for the familiar object. This data confirms that METH results in cognitive memory deficits probably due to sustained adverse neurodevelopmental effects. Neurochemical analyses of the frontal cortex indicated decreased serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE) levels on PnD35. METH is widely recognised for its pro-inflammatory effects, while the reduced 5-HT levels observed may have been the result of an increase in circulating pro-inflammatory cytokines. Neurochemical analyses provided thought-provoking data concerning the role of the permissive hypotheses of depression, indicating that dopamine (DA) is most likely not responsible for the behavioural effects observed, at least under the current study conditions, whereas 5-HT is decidedly more involved than expected. The data also suggest that depletion in NE plays a role in the development of depressive-like behaviours following METH exposure. Based on these findings, we propose that disturbances in 5-HT and NE are a crucial mechanism in how METH abuse may precipitate or worsen depressive-like symptoms in individuals who abuse METH. It should be noted that this study does not discard the role of DA in the development of depression after METH exposure, although under the current study conditions it appears that DA does not play a central role. The current study demonstrated that pre-adolescent exposure to METH can reproduce most of the behavioural changes seen in depressed individuals, and that these behavioural data can be used to identify causal neurochemical factors. Environmental stressors such as METH abuse should be regarded as an additional diagnostic criterion and is relevant to an accumulative risk factor hypothesis. Furthermore, although further study is required, the data suggests that early-life exposure to METH may predispose an individual to mood disorders and behavioural abnormalities later in life. / MSc (Pharmacology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2015
79

An exploration of the coping strategies of early adolescents in two Gauteng schools / Anneté Nel

Nel, Anneté January 2014 (has links)
In the South African context adolescents need to cope with societal and family-related stressors seen as everyday stressors, such as parental divorce, violence in communities, bereavement and pressure in schools. Research has shown that these everyday stressors could have negative effects on their well-being. It was further seen that such a high exposure to everyday stress like school-based stressors leads to an increase in unhealthy behaviour, such as smoking and alcohol use in early adolescents. It is for this reason that resilience in adolescence is such an important factor. With all the challenges and everyday stress experienced by early adolescents, the wellness and resiliency perspective adds a positive angle to adolescent functioning. Constructively coping with everyday stress can therefore lead towards well-being. The purpose of this study was to explore how early adolescents cope with everyday stress. A qualitative research method was used with an embedded case study research design. A voluntary sample was drawn, consisting of 15 South African early adolescents from two schools in Gauteng (ages 12 to 15 years; boys and girls). Data on the subjective experience of coping strategies with everyday stress was gathered through individual semi-structured interviews (ten participants) and drawings to aid the verbalisation of their coping strategies. A focus group discussion with a different set of five participants added to the richness of data and to crystallisation, as they discussed themes which emerged from the individual interviews. Data were analysed thematically and visually. The results indicated that early adolescents experienced different types of everyday stress which accordingly needed a variety of coping strategies. Participants coped using intrapersonal resources (existential belief, creative activities, cognitive coping), interpersonal strengths (family, friends, social media) and behaviour-focussed coping (physical activity, fine motor behaviour). These coping strategies linked with the six modalities outlined in Lahad’s BASIC Ph resiliency model, namely beliefs, affect, social interaction, imagery, cognition and physical activity. Of special interest was the clear indication of the role of social media as part of their coping strategies. / MA (Research Psychology), North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2014
80

Effects of iron and omega–3 fatty acid supplementation on physical activity of iron deficient primary school children residing in KwaZulu–Natal / Greeff J.

Greeff, Jani January 2011 (has links)
Background: Iron deficiency (ID) is the most prevalent nutritional deficiency in the world. In children, both inadequate iron and fatty acid (FA) status have been found to have an effect on cognitive and behavioural function, including physical activity behaviour and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)–related behaviour. Aim: To investigate the effects of supplementation with iron and omega–3 fatty acids (n–3 FAs), alone and in combination, on spontaneous motor activity and ADHD–related behaviour in iron deficient primary school children in KwaZulu–Natal. An additional aim was to evaluate the use of the Actical accelerometer as a tool to assess physical activity behaviour. Methods: The study design was a 2x2 factorial, randomized, double–blind and placebo–controlled trial. Iron deficient school children aged six to ten years with or without mild anaemia were included in the study (n = 321). Subjects were randomly assigned to receive one of the following supplement combinations: (1) 420mg docosahexaenoic acid (DHA)/80 mg eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) + 50mg of iron as ferrous sulphate (Fe); (2) 420mg DHA/80mg EPA + placebo; (3) 50mg of Fe + placebo; (4) placebo + placebo. Supplements were provided four times a week for a duration of 8.5 months (excluding school holidays). Physical activity of a subgroup of subjects (n=98) was recorded on four random school days at baseline, midpoint and endpoint (12 days in total) during three different time periods namely class time 1 (08h00-10h30), break time (10h30–11h00) and class time 2 (11h00–12h00). Classroom behaviour of study subjects was assessed by teachers at baseline and endpoint using the Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale–Revised: Short Forms (CTRS). Iron status indicators and red blood cell (RBC) FA composition were measured at baseline and endpoint. Treatment effects were assessed for activity and CTRS scores. Furthermore, the relationship between activity, CTRS scores and iron/FA status indicators was determined using bivariate correlation and multivariate linear regression analysis. Results: Overall activity of all subjects varied over time from baseline and midpoint to endpoint. A significant cycle x age interaction (P = 0.005) as well as a significant cycle x time period x gender interaction (P = 0.036) was observed on overall activity. There were no significant interactions of cycle or time period with treatment. However, there was a significant main effect of DHA/EPA supplementation for lower class time 1 activity at endpoint (P = 0.014). Biological markers indicating better or poorer iron status were positively and negatively associated with activity at break time, respectively. Subjects in the group receiving both iron and DHA/EPA supplements showed a significant improvement from baseline to endpoint on the cognitive problems/inattention subscale (P = 0.005) of the CTRS. Hyperactivity scores increased iv significantly from baseline to endpoint in all groups (P = 0.006). DHA (r = –.203; P = 0.040) and EPA (r = –.199; P = 0.044) content of RBC were negatively associated with activity at class time 1. No significant associations were observed between activity and CTRS scores at baseline. At endpoint, class time 1 activity was positively associated with all CTRS subscale scores except for the cognitive problems subscale, which only bordered significance (correlation, P = 0.051; regression, P = 0.073). Conclusions: These findings suggest that n–3 FA supplementation may have an influence on ADHD–related behaviour during class time. During school break time when subjects were allowed to move around freely, iron status was positively associated with spontaneous motor activity. Furthermore, the accelerometer might be a useful complimentary tool for assessing both classroom and break time activity behaviour in school children. / Thesis (M.Sc. (Nutrition))--North-West University, Potchefstroom Campus, 2012.

Page generated in 0.0376 seconds