• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 78
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 90
  • 49
  • 48
  • 48
  • 16
  • 16
  • 12
  • 12
  • 10
  • 9
  • 9
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 7
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
41

Charting a parallel course : meeting the community service needs of persons with spinal injuries /

Kemp. Lynn Amanda. January 1999 (has links)
Thesis (Ph.D.)--University of Western Sydney, Macarthur, Faculty of Health, 1999. / References: p. 194-201.
42

Place attachment and MacArthur Park a case study of the importance of public space in an immigrant neighborhood and the implications for local planning practice /

Main, Kelly Diane, January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--UCLA, 2007. / Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 329-340).
43

The Truman-MacArthur controversy, a study in political-military relations

Potter, Allen R. January 1972 (has links)
Thesis (M.M.A.S.)--U.S. Army Command and General Staff College. / Includes bibliographical references (p. 200-203).
44

Desenvolvimento lexical inicial dos 8 aos 16 meses de idade a partir do inventário MacArthur de desenvolvimento comunicativo – protocolo palavras e gestos

Silva, Cláudia Tereza Sobrinho da January 2003 (has links)
Submitted by Glauber Assunção Moreira (glauber.a.moreira@gmail.com) on 2018-09-12T19:01:23Z No. of bitstreams: 1 O DESENVOLVIMENTO LEXICAL INICIAL DOS 8 AOS 16 MESES DE IDADE A PARTIR DO INVENTÁRIO MACARTHUR DE DESENVOLVIMENTO COMUNICATIVO – PROTOCOLO PALAVRAS E GESTOS.pdf: 1929876 bytes, checksum: 697c5acd6e5a80c23aee19c6d81b29bf (MD5) / Approved for entry into archive by Marly Santos (marly@ufba.br) on 2018-09-13T17:10:34Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 O DESENVOLVIMENTO LEXICAL INICIAL DOS 8 AOS 16 MESES DE IDADE A PARTIR DO INVENTÁRIO MACARTHUR DE DESENVOLVIMENTO COMUNICATIVO – PROTOCOLO PALAVRAS E GESTOS.pdf: 1929876 bytes, checksum: 697c5acd6e5a80c23aee19c6d81b29bf (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2018-09-13T17:10:34Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 O DESENVOLVIMENTO LEXICAL INICIAL DOS 8 AOS 16 MESES DE IDADE A PARTIR DO INVENTÁRIO MACARTHUR DE DESENVOLVIMENTO COMUNICATIVO – PROTOCOLO PALAVRAS E GESTOS.pdf: 1929876 bytes, checksum: 697c5acd6e5a80c23aee19c6d81b29bf (MD5) / O desenvolvimento lexical inicial de crianças entre 8 e 16 meses de idade adquirindo o português brasileiro é um assunto pouco explorado pela literatura. Adotando como instrumento de coleta o protocolo “Palavras e Gestos” dos Inventários MacArthur de Desenvolvimento Comunicativo – elaborado em língua inglesa e já adaptado e normatizado para diversos idiomas – que se refere à medição da compreensão e da produção lexical e ao uso de gestos comunicativos de crianças desta faixa etária, e adotando também a sua metodologia, baseada no relato parental, o presente trabalho tem como objetivos: fornecer informações a respeito dos padrões de compreensão e de produção lexical, a partir das informações que podem ser obtidas com o protocolo, no intuito de fornecer uma visão do desenvolvimento lexical dos 8 aos 16 meses de idade; adaptar o protocolo para o português brasileiro a fim de torná-lo eficaz e válido também para o português; e investigar a eficácia do Inventário enquanto instrumento de pesquisa elaborado para este fim. Para a composição do corpus, 27 pais, pertencentes à classe sócio-escolar A ou B, de crianças nesta faixa etária, foram entrevistados. Os dados obtidos permitiram chegar às seguintes conclusões: existe uma asssimetria entre a quantidade de itens lexicais comprendidos e a quantidade de itens produzidos; no nível da compreensão, o grupo dos “Nomes Comuns” predomina em todas as faixas etárias, seguido do grupo dos “Predicados”; na produção, as categorias “Sons de Coisas e de Aninais”, “Pessoas”, “Objetos e Lugares fora da casa” e “Jogos e Rotinas” dominam até o primeiro ano de vida, sendo que, a partir daí, outras categorias semânticas como “Comidas e Bebidas”, “Palavras de Ação”, “Partes do Corpo”, “Animais” e “Utensílios da Casa” começam a aparecer em maior número. Algumas informações importantes e necessárias para uma análise desta fase inicial como contextos e freqüência de uso e forma fonética não são fornecidas devido a limitações do próprio intrumento. Em linhas gerais, as informações oriundas desta coleta corroboram estudos já realizados, em outras línguas, a respeito dessa fase inicial de aquisição, embora os dados obtidos com o protocolo sejam considerados elevados em comparação com os dados de outros estudos longitudinais e de diário. / The early lexical development of children between 8 to 16 months acquiring the brazilian portuguese is a poorly explored topic in the avaiable literature. Adopting as a data-gathering model the “Words and Gestures” protocol of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories – developed for the English language but alredy widely adapted and normalized for use with various languages – wich aims to measure the comprehension, lexical production and the use of communicative gestures by children in this age range, and adopting as well the metodology, based in parental report, this dissertation hopes to achieve the following objectives: to provide further data regarding the patterns of comprehension and production in the lexical development, within the limits of the protocol, with the goal of developing a vision of the lexical development of children fron 8 to 16 months; to adapt the protocol validly and effectively for use with brazilian portuguese; and to investigate the effectiveness of the Inventory as an instrument to achieve these goals. The body of data was composed by interviews with 27 parents of children in this age range, either from socio-educational class A or B. the data obtained suggest the following conclusions: that the comprehension of lexical items occurs asymmetrically with regards to production; that at the level of comprehension, the group “Common Nouns” predominates at all ages, followed by the group of “Predicatives”; that in production the categories “Sounds of Things and Animals”, “People”, “Object and Places outside ”, and “Games and Routines” predominate up to one year of age, but after that other semantic categories such as “Foods and Drinks”, “Action Words”, “Parts of the Body”, “Animals”, and “Household Items” begin to appear with greater frequency. Some important and necessary information for an in depth analysis at this initial state, such as the contexts and frequencies of use and the phonetic forms utilized are not supplied due to the limitations of the model for data collection employed. Generally, the data from this study corroborate previous research performed in other languages regarding this initial satge of language acquisition, although the data obtained with the protocol may be considered high in comparison with data provided by other studies.
45

A derelication of duty : Douglas MacArthur and the development of the Philippine military

Beitelman, Phillip C. 01 January 2010 (has links)
Was Douglas MacArthur directly responsible for the ineffectiveness of the Philippine military during the Philippine campaign of 1941-1942? This question is still a point of debate among historians. During the six years that preceded the United States entrance into the Second World War, the government of the Commonwealth of the Philippines attempted to implement a national defense plan devised by Douglas MacArthur. The end goal of MacArthur's plan was the creation of a professional Filipino military force that would be able to adequately defend the Philippines against invasion. From late 1935 until mid 1941 , MacArthur served as the official United States military adviser to the Commonwealth government. When war with Japan occurred in late 1941 the Philippine military was in dismal shape. The argument that MacArthur was directly responsible for the ineffectiveness of the Philippine military in the Philippine campaign of 1941-1942 is strongly supported by historical evidence of events that occurred during the creation, development, and fielding of the Philippine military in 1934-1941. An investigation of historical evidence shows that MacArthur is responsible for making a series of errors that caused rampant financial problems and an inadequate training program. These factors directly contributed to the ineffectiveness of Philippine military units during the 1941-1942 campaign.
46

Discerning missional criteria for the practice of church planting by the South MacArthur Church of Christ

Ogren, John Albert. January 2006 (has links)
Thesis (D. Min.)--Abilene Christian University, 2006. / Abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 116-120).
47

The translation of irony in Australian political commentary texts from English into Arabic /

Chakhachiro, Raymond. January 1997 (has links)
Thesis (Ph.D.)--University of Western Sydney, Macarthur, [Faculty of Education], 1997. / Bibliography: p. 219-226.
48

Comparative study of production, infectivity, and effectiveness of arbuscular mycorrhizal fungi produced by soil-based and soil-less techniques /

Asif, Mohammad. January 1997 (has links)
Thesis (Ph.D.)-- University of Western Sydney, Macarthur, Dept. of Biological Sciences, 1997.
49

An investigation of long-term dependence in time-series data /

Ellis, Craig. January 1998 (has links)
Thesis: Ph.D.--University of Western Sydney, Macarthur.Faculty of Business and Technology. 1998.
50

Some effects of air-filled porosity on the suppression of damping-off of seedlings by pythium ultimatum in compost amended potting media /

Lainà, Rosetta. January 1997 (has links)
Thesis: M.Sc. (Hons.)--University of Western Sydney, Macarthur. Faculty of Business and Technology. 1997. / References: 141-153.

Page generated in 0.0315 seconds