Spelling suggestions: "subject:"prenatal influences."" "subject:"prenatale influences.""
1 |
The effects of maternal stress on offspring behaviorStephens, Margaret 05 1900 (has links)
No description available.
|
2 |
Prenatal attitudes and maternal perception of the newborn infantGlass, Jennifer L. January 1979 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison. / Typescript. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 64-66).
|
3 |
A data collection system for the prenatal genetics programMcMahon, Neil G. January 1979 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison. / Typescript. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 36-39).
|
4 |
Very early experience the effects of prenatal injections of low levels of DDT on the postnatal behavior of chicks.Novak, Melinda A. January 1971 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--University of Wisconsin--Madison, 1971. / eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references.
|
5 |
A comparison of selected livebirths over five and one-half pounds at birth with livebirths five and one-half pounds or less at birthColwell, Frederick Herbert, January 1946 (has links)
Thesis--Univ. of Michigan. / Bibliography: leaves 106-112.
|
6 |
Anxiety, personality and birth deliveryBanner, Ronald Harlan. January 1968 (has links)
Dissertation (Ph.D.)--Colorado State University, 1968. / Typescript (photocopy) Bibliography: leaves [101]-106.
|
7 |
Skeletal growth and development of the human fetus : effect of maternal and nutritional factors.Roberts, Jill Anne January 1971 (has links)
Growth is associated with the availability of essential nutrients and it seems possible that these nutrients could affect the growth mechanism involved in skeletal development. To test this hypothesis 76 normal human fetuses aged 9 to 20 weeks were collected from therapeutic abortions. Sex, weight, length, head circumference, foot length and a skeletal index were recorded; developmental age was calculated from crown-rump length, and gestational age estimated from the mother's menstrual history.
Bones from the right arm and leg were removed and cleaned for biochemical analysis. Calcium, inorganic phosphorus, magnesium, sodium and collagen content of 60 femora and humeri were determined, after length, fresh weight, constant dry weight and fat-free weight were recorded. Length of ossification in the bones of the left arm and leg was measured via silver radiography. Assuming bilateral symmetry, biochemical and physical data could then be compared.
All fetal data were grouped according to developmental age: 9-10, 11-12, 13-14, 15-16, 17-20 weeks. Analysis of variance and Duncan's New Multiple Range Test were performed to determine the significance of group effect. Simple linear regression was executed on the whole range of data to detect which variables best predicted other variables.
Maternal information was obtained from an interview and from medical records at Vancouver General Hospital. Age, weight, height, birth weight, parity and gravidity of the mother were recorded. A socio-economic index was calculated. Adequacy of maternal diet during pregnancy was assessed from a daily pattern recall, food frequency and preference questions. These data were used to calculate a total nutrition score and a protein score. Maternal data were coded as potential independent variables and multiple regression analysis performed against fetal dependent variables.
As developmental age of the fetuses increased, the fresh length, dry weight and length of ossification also increased in both humerus and femur, as did the calcium and phosphorus content. In most cases long bone growth as measured by these variables advanced proportionately with fetal age. Thus group means of most variables were significantly different from each other when divided into five 2 week age periods. Water content dropped proportionately with age, reflecting bone mineralization. Sodium content fell markedly in fetal bones after 10 weeks. Magnesium and collagen remained constant. Fat extraction did not change the dry weight of the bones. Statistical correlation was found between physical and biochemical data. Generally physical variables were best predicted by other physical variables. Biochemical composition of the femur could best be predicted from corresponding data in the humerus. When gestational age was plotted against physical or biochemical variables, statistical correlation was weaker.
The correlation found between fetal variables and maternal age, parity, weight and socio-economic status would indicate a diversity of factors influencing fetal growth. Whereas protein score of maternal diet was not statistically related with fetal parameters, general nutrition score showed a consistent, positive correlation with length and dry weight of the femur and humerus. This relationship was statistically significant when developmental or gestational age remained constant. The results of this study suggest that nutrition of the pregnant woman is positively correlated with some indices of skeletal growth and development of the human fetus. / Land and Food Systems, Faculty of / Graduate
|
8 |
The role of prenatal auditory stimulation in the development of filial behaviour in the domestic ducklingDe Wet, John Manning January 1974 (has links)
The aim of the first three experiments was to delineate the physical characteristics of an artificial call for which naive Peking X Aylesbury ducklings show the greatest natural preference. In each experiment SO ducklings were tested at 20 ± 2 hrs. posthatching, for following and approach responses to one of four auditory stimulus conditions or a silent model. The responsiveness of subjects was greatest when the call contained tone frequencies of 500, 800, and 1600Hz and was presented at a repetition rate of 4/second with a note duration of 50 milliseconds (Optimal call). In Experiment 4 Peking X Aylesbury eggs were exposed to intermittent prenatal stimulation with the optimal call and the hatchlings were tested for responsiveness to this call at 20 ± 2 hrs. posthatching. Subjects with prenatal auditory experience of the optimal call showed significantly greater responsiveness to this call than non-stimulated control subjects. The aim of the final experiment was to determine whether the natural auditory stimulus preferences of ducklings could be overridden through sheer prenatal experience of a non-preferred call. Ducklings with prenatal experience of the non-preferred call continued to show as strong a preference for the optimal call as non stimulated control subjects. The responses of both stimulated and non-stimulated subjects to the optimal call were significantly stronger than the responses of stimulated and non-stimulated subjects to the non-preferred call. The responses of stimulated subjects to the non-preferred call were only slightly stronger than responses of non-stimulated subjects to the same call. These results indicate that responsiveness to calls for which naive birds show the greatest preference is enhanced by prenatal experience of the preferred call. This effect is not evident, however, when subjects are stimulated prenatally with a non-preferred call.
|
9 |
Effect of prenatal energy intake, monensin and hemophilus somnus vaccination on serum immunoglobulin values of suckling beef calvesVoisin, Alain J January 2011 (has links)
Typescript (photocopy). / Digitized by Kansas Correctional Industries
|
10 |
Dietary consistency and habits and affective nutritive processes in their relation to the development of specificity, including speechRoss, Hattie Bell, January 1900 (has links)
Résumé of Thesis (Sc. D.)--University of Michigan, 1940. / Reproduced from type-written copy. Bibliography: leaf 6. Also issued in print.
|
Page generated in 0.0796 seconds