301 |
Do Chinese children with developmental dyslexia have deficit in visualtemporal processing?馮錦珊, Fung, Kam-shan. January 2008 (has links)
published_or_final_version / Educational Psychology / Master / Master of Social Sciences
|
302 |
Dysfunction in emotional decoding in autistic spectrum disorder: theory-of-mind impairment or weak centralcoherence蔡慧怡, Choi, Wae-yee, Wendy. January 2008 (has links)
published_or_final_version / Educational Psychology / Master / Master of Social Sciences
|
303 |
The cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in young children with autism盧定欣, Lo, Ting-yan, Diane. January 2008 (has links)
published_or_final_version / Educational Psychology / Master / Master of Social Sciences
|
304 |
Do Chinese children with developmental dyslexia have deficit in auditory temporal processing?翟建華, Chak, Kin-wah, Christopher. January 2008 (has links)
published_or_final_version / Educational Psychology / Master / Master of Social Sciences
|
305 |
Validation of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders-revised (SCARED-R), and a study on the relationship among temperamental traits, attentional bias and anxiety in children and adolescentsSze, Mei-lun, Angela, 施美倫 January 2014 (has links)
Introduction: This study was designed to validate the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorder-Revised (SCARED-R) for use in the local context. Furthermore, different components of anxiety-related attentional bias were studied with emotional spatial cueing task (ESCT). This study also attempted to investigate the relationship among attentional bias, temperamental traits of Negative Affectivity (NA) and Effortful Control (EC), and anxiety.
Method: Subjects were recruited from P.4 to F.3 students of mainstream Chinese speaking schools. The SCARED-R and self-report measures of NA and EC were administered. 508 child/adolescent subjects (mean age=11.7 years, SD=1.80; 327 boys and 181 girls) and 312 of their parents (child’s mean age=11.50; SD=1.77; 196 boys and 116 girls) were included in the analysis. In Stage 2 of the study, subjects were recruited from those with SCARED-R scores at 85th percentile or above and 50th percentile or below. 34 pairs of child/adolescent and parent subjects (mean age=11.89 years; SD=1.90) were assessed with the Chinese version of the Diagnostic Interview for Children-Version 4, with which the Anxiety group (n=14) and Control group (n=20) were identified. All subjects completed the SCARED-R and Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS). The child/adolescent subjects did ESCT, which presented Angry, happy and neutral faces for 17 ms, 500 ms and 1250 ms.
Results: The mean total SCARED-R scores were 31.94 (SD=19.51) and 22.96 (SD=15.35) for child/adolescent and parent subjects. Internal consistencies (Cronbach’s α=.95 and .94) and one-month test-retest reliability (Spearman’s rho=.72 and .79, both at p<.001) for child and parent total SCARED-R scores were satisfactory. Total SCARED-R scores correlated significantly with total SCAS scores, and were significantly different between the Anxiety and Control groups. These provided support for SCARED-R’s convergent validity. Results of exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses suggested that SCARED-R was multi-dimensional.
Facilitated attentional bias and disengagement difficulties were not related as predicted. Angry cues induced greater disengagement difficulties than happy cues. Compared with Control group, Anxiety group showed greater disengagement difficulties when Angry cues were presented at cue duration of 1250 ms.
A direct and significant relationship existed between anxiety with NA, and between anxiety and EC. NA and EC played their own unique roles in affecting anxiety but the variance accounted by EC in addition to NA was small. EC did not moderate the relationship between NA and anxiety, though a very small mediating effect between NA and anxiety was noted.
Conclusion: This study provides support for the satisfactory psychometric properties of the SCARED-R for local use, showing its good potential of being used as a screening instrument for anxiety disorders. Despite the small sample size, anxious children/adolescents were found to have disengagement difficulties from threat cue at cue duration of 1250 ms, and this could be a parameter to consider in future design of attentional bias modification training. The major limitation of this study is its convenience sampling of subjects from community and the low participation/consent rate. This limits generalizability of the results, impedes an investigation of the sensitivity and specificity of the scale, and poses great constraints on statistical power. / published_or_final_version / Psychiatry / Doctoral / Doctor of Philosophy
|
306 |
The effect of selected socio-environmental variables on the dietary intake of preschool childrenCondit, Mary Garidel 25 February 1976 (has links)
This investigation was undertaken to obtain information on the
dietary intake of Oregon preschool children and to determine the
effect of certain socio-environmental variables on the nutrient intake
of these children. Three hundred boys and girls who were attending
Well Child or Multiphasic Screening Clinics in 11 counties participated
in this project. A home economist interviewed the parent to obtain
the child's dietary intake and socio-environmental data of the family.
Dietary intake was determined by 24-hour recall or 3-day records.
Nutrient intake was compared to the 1974 Recommended Dietary
Allowances (RDA); intakes below 67 percent of the RDA were considered
low. Correlations of socio-environmental factors with
nutrient intake were determined by simple linear regression and
multiple regression analysis.
The mean and median of the children's intake of vitamin A, exceeded the RDA. While the median of their niacin intake was just
below the RDA, the mean and median of their iron intake was below
the recommended level. Protein and riboflavin were consumed
abundantly, with 0 and 3.3 percent of the subjects, respectively,
failing to receive two-thirds of the RDA. Sixty-three percent of the
children ingested diets that supplied two to three times the RDA for
protein. The nutrients most commonly lacking were iron, ascorbic
acid, calcium and thiamin, with 39.7, 18.7, 15.3, and 13.0 percent,
respectively, of the children having low intakes. Forty-one percent
of the children received a nutrient supplement, which was not included
in these calculations.
The percentage of children consuming diets containing less than
67 percent of the RDA increased with age for the intakes of vitamin A,
thiamin, and kilocalories. Low intakes of iron, on the other hand,
declined with age. Riboflavin intake was lowest among the four- to
six-year-old children. Adequacy of ascorbic acid, niacin and calcium
did not show any relation to age.
The socio-environmental variables examined were geographic
location (urban, rural, or metropolitan), number of siblings as well as
education and occupation of each parent. Other socio-environmental
variables considered were whether or not the family received public
assistance or had contact with the Cooperative Extension Service
personnel and bulletins. When multiple regression analysis was
ascorbic acid, thiamin, riboflavin, calcium, protein, and kilocalories performed, the only significant correlation (p < 0.05) was observed
between the intake of ascorbic acid, mother's education, and mother's
profession. Four socio-environmental variables were significantly
correlated (p < 0.05) with the intake of one or more nutrients by
simple linear regression. A significant correlation was found between
mother's education and the intakes of ascorbic acid, calories, and
thiamin. A significant correlation was observed between mother's
profession and the intakes of niacin and ascorbic acid. Father's
education was found to be significantly related to the intake of thiamin.
The only significant, negative correlation was found between the intake
of thiamin and the number of siblings. / Graduation date: 1976
|
307 |
Communication and language development in deaf and hearing infantsRobinshaw, Helen Mary January 1992 (has links)
No description available.
|
308 |
Perverting motherhood? : sexuality and lesbian parent familiesGabb, Jacqui January 2002 (has links)
No description available.
|
309 |
Growing up with 'moderniteÌ' : representations of childhood and adolescence in French painting, 1848-1886Green, Anna January 2002 (has links)
No description available.
|
310 |
Nutritional factors associated with obesity in adolescent females in the United Arab EmiratesAl Hourani, Huda Mustafa January 2002 (has links)
No description available.
|
Page generated in 0.0673 seconds