• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 74
  • 11
  • 10
  • 7
  • 5
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 163
  • 163
  • 43
  • 33
  • 31
  • 30
  • 24
  • 24
  • 22
  • 21
  • 19
  • 18
  • 16
  • 15
  • 15
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
11

Dysphoria and facial emotion recognition: Examining the role of rumination

Duong, David January 2012 (has links)
Rumination has been shown to be an influential part of the depressive experience, impacting on various cognitive processes including memory and attention. However, there is a dearth of studies examining the relationship between rumination and emotion recognition, deficits or biases in which have been closely linked to a depressive mood state. In Study 1, participants (N = 89) received either a rumination or distraction induction prior to completing three variants of an emotion recognition task assessing decoding accuracy or biases. Results demonstrated that greater levels of dysphoria were associated with poorer facial emotion recognition accuracy, but only when participants were induced to ruminate (as opposed to being induced to distract). The aim of Study 2 (N = 172) was to examine a possible mechanism, namely cognitive load, by which rumination affects emotion recognition. Results from this study indicated that participants endorsing greater levels of dysphoria were less accurate on an emotion recognition task when they received either a rumination induction or a cognitive load task compared to their counterparts who received a distraction induction. Importantly, the performance of those in the cognitive load and rumination conditions did not differ from each other. In summary, these findings suggest that the confluence of dysphoria and rumination can influence individuals’ accuracy in identifying emotional content portrayed in facial expressions. Furthermore, rumination, by definition an effortful process, might negatively impact emotion recognition via the strain it places on cognitive resources.
12

Genetic and Parental Influences on the Development of Emotion Recognition Skills in Children

John, Sufna Gheyara 01 August 2014 (has links)
AN ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION OF SUFNA GHEYARA JOHN, for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Psychology, presented on March 21st, 2014, at Southern Illinois University Carbondale. TITLE: GENETIC AND PARENTAL INFLUENCES ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTION RECOGNITION SKILLS IN CHILDREN MAJOR PROFESSOR: Dr. Lisabeth DiLalla The purpose of this study was to examine the magnitude of genetic and environmental influences on children's emotion recognition (ER) skills and social difficulties (bullying and victimization). An additional goal was to examine the relation between parent ER skills, child ER skills, and child social difficulties. It was expected that genetic and environmental influences would account for a significant portion of the variance in child ER skills and social difficulties and that child ER skills and social difficulties would share common genetic and environmental influences. Moreover, it was predicted that parent and child ER skills would significantly predict child social difficulties. Finally, it was predicted that child angry and fearful biases in ER abilities would lead to greater social difficulties. 121 children (forming 69 twin pairs) ages 6-10 years and their parents participated in the study. Children and their parents completed an objective measure of ER abilities and subjective measures of child social difficulties. Separate analyses were conducted for child social difficulties by informant (parent or child) and type of difficulty (bullying or victimization). Results from this study suggest that genetic and non-shared environmental influences account for a significant portion of the variance in child ER skills, parent-reported bullying and victimization, and child-reported bullying. Conversely, environmental influences account for a significant portion of the variance in child-reported victimization. Child ER abilities and child-reported bullying shared common genetic influences. Path modeling demonstrated that parent ER skills predicted child ER skills and parent-reported bullying, whereas child ER skills predicted child-reported victimization. Finally, children who demonstrated an angry or fearful bias had greater involvement in bullying and were more victimized. These results underscore the importance of conceptualizing bullying and victimization from a biopsychosocial perspective that incorporates both biological and environmental influences on complex social behavior. Moreover, results in this study varied by informant, suggesting that it is important to gather information from multiple perspectives in order to gain the most comprehensive view of child behavior. Finally, these results suggest that helping children to achieve a more balanced perspective in their emotion recognition abilities may help reduce their involvement in socially maladaptive interactions.
13

Facial Emotion Recognition and Reflexive Facial Mimicry in Individuals with a History of Non-suicidal Self-injury

Ziebell, Laura 19 March 2021 (has links)
Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) has been regarded as a dysfunctional coping strategy for managing intensely difficult feelings and is a growing area of concern in clinical and non-clinical populations alike. Individuals who engage in NSSI often report significant interpersonal difficulties, with studies showing that they have impaired social interactions. Attending to the emotional expressions of others is important for appropriate social functioning, and subtly mimicking the expressions of others is an unconscious behaviour that encourages empathy and interpersonal bonding. Differences in emotion recognition and reflection can impact social behaviour, yet little research has assessed how individuals with a history of NSSI (HNSSI) process facial expressions of emotion. In this thesis, the results of three studies designed to investigate potential differences in emotion recognition and emotion mimicry in individuals with a history of NSSI compared to controls are reported. Results from Study 1 revealed that when presented with colourful, dynamic morphing stimuli showing emotional expressions, HNSSI participants were able to correctly categorize negative expressions like anger, disgust, sadness, and the ambiguous emotion of surprise at a lower stimulus intensity compared to controls; They also correctly categorized fear with greater accuracy, though at the same intensity as controls. However, in Study 2, when static, greyscale images were obscured with varying levels of fractal noise, HNSSI individuals did not show superior signal-proportion thresholds. These results may suggest that higher-order elements of visual and cognitive processing are evoked by more realistic social stimuli. In the third study, HNSSI participants were found to produce significantly less electrical activity in the corrugator muscle in response to viewing angry stimuli, and significantly less of an expected relaxation in muscle activity in response to viewing happy stimuli. Complementing these results, it was found that endorsing social influence as a motivator for engaging in NSSI was associated with less mimicry, whereas endorsing emotion regulation as a motivator was associated with greater incongruent muscle response when viewing happy faces. These observed differences in facial mimicry between HNSSI and controls may be related to some of the observed relationship difficulties experienced by this group. Results from this research may help us to better understand NSSI behaviour, as well as help to inform and further develop therapies intended to address it.
14

The Role of Dispositional Mindfulness in the Development of Emotion Recognition Ability and Inhibitory Control from Late Adolescence to Early Adulthood

Dawson, Glen C. 02 September 2020 (has links)
No description available.
15

FACIAL EMOTION RECOGNITION IN GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER AND DEPRESSION: ASSESSING FOR UNIQUE AND COMMON RESPONSES TO EMOTIONS AND NEUTRALITY

Linardatos, Eftihia 30 November 2011 (has links)
No description available.
16

Maternal attachment and recognition of infant emotion

Riley, Helen January 2014 (has links)
Objective: The overall aim of this study was to investigate whether maternal emotion recognition of infant faces in a facial morphing task differed by maternal attachment style, and if this was moderated by a secure attachment prime, such that it would ameliorate the effects of maternal attachment insecurity. Method: 87 mothers of children aged 0-18 months completed measures of global and mother-specific trait attachment, post-natal depression, mood and state attachment alongside 2 sessions of an emotion recognition task. This task was made up of short movies created from photographs of infant faces, changing from neutral to either happy or sad. It was designed to assess sensitivity (accuracy of responses and intensity of emotion required to recognize the emotion) to changes in emotions expressed in the faces of infants. Participants also underwent a prime manipulation that was either attachment-based (experimental group) or neutral (control group). Results: There were no significant effects for global attachment scores (i.e., avoidant, anxious). However, there was a significant interaction effect of condition x maternal avoidant attachment for accuracy of recognition of happy infant faces. Explication of this interaction yielded an unexpected finding: participants reporting avoidant attachment relationships with their own mothers were less accurate in recognizing happy infant faces following the attachment prime than participants with maternal avoidant attachment in the control condition. Conclusions: Future research directions suggest ways to improve strength of effects and variability in attachment insecurity. Clinical implications of the study center on the preliminary evidence presented that supports carefully selected and executed interventions for mothers with attachment problems.
17

Emotion recognition in parents attending Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services

Donnelly, Katherine January 2015 (has links)
Objectives: This study sought to determine whether a computerised cognitive bias modification programme could be effective within a waiting-room setting for parents accompanying their children to CAMHS appointments. The primary objectives were to determine whether detectable changes to participants' emotion recognition could be observed in this setting, and whether this approach would be acceptable to the population. Secondary measures investigated whether the programme would lead to changes in participants' affect or changes in parents' appraisals of difficulties with children. Methods: A computerised emotion recognition training task was delivered to all participants during four weekly sessions. Participants in the experimental condition (n=17) received feedback aiming to shift their detection of positive facial emotions, while those in the control condition (n=14) received feedback which was not designed to elicit any shift in emotion detection. Results: Positive shifts in emotion recognition were observed in the experimental group, although no changes were observed in secondary measures in either control or experimental groups. Qualitative data indicated that the programme was acceptable and appropriately constructed. Conclusion: This study demonstrates that cognitive bias modification is possible within a waiting-room setting, although the extent to which this can lead to clinically significant improvements in mood or relationships remains uncertain. This work has implications for emotion recognition interventions for clinical populations known to present with negative emotional biases (e.g. anxiety and depression) and represents an important first research step towards developing interventions to improve parent-child relationships.
18

Associations between TBI, facial emotion recognition, impulse control and aggression in delinquent and vulnerable young people

Tanskanen, Sanna-Leena January 2015 (has links)
Objectives: There is evidence that childhood traumatic brain injury (TBI) is associated with increased risk of offending and violent crime. This study aimed to explore associations between TBI in a group of delinquent and vulnerable young people (VYP) at risk of offending, and facial emotion recognition (FER) abilities, inhibition control (Stop-IT) and self-reported reactive-proactive aggression (RPQ). Methods: There were two studies. The first study used a cross sectional between group design to compare 45 VYP (with and without TBI) and a control group of 59 students on FER task measuring emotion recognition accuracy of six basic emotions. The second study examined differences between TBI and non-TBI groups in the VYP sample (N=21) on a Stop-IT task, FER accuracy and self-reported reactive-proactive aggression. Results: A history of TBI was reported by 60% of the VYP group (48.9% with loss of consciousness [LoC]), whereas 30% of the control group reported a history of TBI (25.4% with LoC). The VYP group (with and without TBI) demonstrated a similar pattern of reduced overall FER accuracy that was significantly different to the control group. Compared to the control group, The VYP groups (with and without TBI) were less accurate on recognising anger, disgust, sadness and surprise, but not happy and fear. There were no significant differences between the TBI- and non-TBI groups. The second study did not find any significant differences between the TBI and non-TBI groups on overall FER accuracy, Stop-IT performance, and RPQ scores. There were also no significant associations between these measures. Conclusions: Future research requires larger samples that enable investigating the association between different severity of TBI, FER and inhibition control ability. Better and more youth-friendly measures are also needed.
19

Emotion processing and social cognition in deaf children

Jones, Anna January 2013 (has links)
Understanding others’ emotions and false beliefs, known as Theory of Mind (ToM), and to recognise and produce facial expressions of emotion has been linked to social competence. Deaf children born to hearing parents have commonly shown a deficit, or at best a delay in ToM. The emotion processing skills of deaf children are less clear. The main aims of this thesis were to clarify the ability of emotion recognition in deaf children, and to provide the first investigation in emotion production. While deaf children were poorer than hearing controls at recognising expressions of emotion in cartoon faces, a similar pattern was found in both groups’ recognition of real human faces of the six basic emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, disgust and surprise). For deaf children, emotion recognition was better in dynamic rather than static, and intense rather than subtle, displays of emotion. With the exception of disgust, no differences in individual emotions were found, suggesting that the use of ecologically valid dynamic real faces facilitates deaf children’s emotion recognition. Deaf children’s ability to produce the six basic emotions was compared to hearing children by videoing voluntary encodings of facial expression elicited via verbal labels and emotion signed stories, and the imitation of dynamic displays of real facial expressions of emotion. With the exception of a poorer performance in imitation and the verbally elicited production of disgust, deaf children were consistently rated by human judges overall as producing more recognisable and intense expressions, suggesting that clarity and expressiveness may be important to deaf individuals’ emotion display rules. In line with previous studies, results showed a delay in passing the first and second order belief tasks in comparison to age matched controls, but not in comparison to a group of ‘age appropriate’ hearing control children. These findings encouragingly suggest that while deaf children of hearing parents show a delay in ToM and understanding disgust, emotion processing skills follow a broadly similar pattern of development to hearing control children. Language experience is implicated in difficulties faced in social and emotion cognition, with reduced opportunities to discuss more complex emotional and mental states.
20

Rastreamento de olhar e reconhecimento de emoções em crianças com transtorno do espectro autístico / Eye tracking and emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder

Muñoz, Patricia de Oliveira Lima 24 September 2018 (has links)
O trabalho pioneiro de Darwin, em 1872, constituiu uma das primeiras fontes de informação sobre as emoções dentro da perspectiva evolucionista. Hoje podemos contar com equipamentos de medidas mais sofisticados que nos permitem revisitar alguma das questões básicas por ele formuladas. A presente tese vem contribuir para uma melhor compreensão do reconhecimento de emoções e teoria da mente em crianças com TEA, apresentando analises que possam levar a sugestões de intervenções clínicas futuras. Embora ainda haja inconsistência entre os estudos quanto a qual emoção está mais prejudicada, tem sido relatadas dificuldades na identificação das emoções de medo, raiva, nojo e surpresa. Usamos um eye-tracker com o objetivo de esclarecer como os participantes olham para estímulos. Os participantes foram 40 meninos com TEA e com desenvolvimento típico, equiparados quanto a idade (6 a 14 anos). Foram avaliados para classificação do QI e para o grau de autismo. Os estímulos visuais foram retirados dos subteste de Reconhecimento de Emoções e Teoria da Mente do NEPSY II e apresentados em um equipamento de rastreamento de olhar, tendo sido definidas áreas de interesse (AI) do rosto. Para cada imagem, foi computado o tempo de fixação (em segundos) em cada AI. Os participantes também realizaram as tarefas do subteste, com o objetivo de verificar o acerto e o erro em cada estímulo. Em nossas análises pudemos verificar que a idade e o QI dos participantes influenciaram o desempenho na tarefa de reconhecimento de emoções e não influenciaram na tarefa de ToM. As crianças com TEA apresentaram um desempenho prejudicado quando comparado com as crianças com DT em ambos subtestes. Os participantes com TEA erraram mais as tarefas com emoções de valência negativa, como tristeza e raiva. A classificação do grau de TEA, não teve influência no desempenho dos participantes. O que parece ocorrer é que pertencer ao grupo clínico TEA seja uma condição para ter um desempenho pior quando comparado com o grupo de DT. Analisando o rastreamento de olhar, constatamos que o grupo DT permaneceu menos tempo fixando o olhar na AI dos olhos e mais tempo na AI da boca. Como o tempo total de fixação para os dois grupos não teve diferença estatística no subteste de reconhecimento de emoção, podemos sugerir que as crianças com TEA apresentam um padrão de olhar que chamamos de \"difuso\" enquanto que as crianças com DT apresentam um padrão de olhar que chamamos de \"focado\" nas regiões da face relevantes para o reconhecimento de emoções. Parece que na tarefa de ToM, o tempo de fixação em olhos e boca não foi o determinante para o erro na resposta e sim o olhar difuso na cena que deveria ser interpretada. Quando avaliamos os resultados qualitativos do rastreamento do olhar podemos pensar na importância de intervenções que possam ajudar crianças com TEA a melhorar seu desempenho em identificar as expressões faciais que auxiliam na decodificação da emoção e na interpretação de estados mentais / Darwin\'s pioneering work in 1872 was one of the earliest sources of information about emotions within the evolutionary perspective. Today we have more sophisticated equipment that allows us to revisit some of the basic questions that he formulates. The present thesis contributed to a better understanding of the emotion recognition and theory of mind in children with ASD, presenting analyzes that may lead to suggestions of future clinical interventions. Although there is still inconsistency among studies as to which emotion is most impaired, difficulties in identifying the emotions of fear, anger, disgust and surprise have been reported. We use an eye-tracker to clarify how participants look at stimuli. The participants were 40 boys with ASD and typical development, matched for age (6 to 14 years). They were evaluated for IQ and for the level of autism. The visual stimuli were from the Emotional Recognition and Theory of Mind subtests of NEPSY II and presented with an eye-tracking device, with areas of interest (AI) defined. For each image, the fixation duration (in seconds) in each AI was computed. The participants also performed the tasks of the subtest, in order to verify the right and wrong answers in each stimulus. In our analyzes we could verify that the participants\' age and IQ influenced the performance in the task of emotion recognition and did not influence the task of ToM. Children with ASD had an impaired performance when compared to children with TD in both subtests. Participants with ASD had a low score in tasks with negative valence emotions, such as sadness and anger. The classification of the ASD level had no influence on the participants\' performance. What seems to occur is that beeing on the ASD clinical group is a condition to had a worse perform when compared to the TD group. Analyzing the eye tracking, we found that the TD group spent less time fixing the eyes in the AI of the eyes and longer in the AI of the mouth. As the total fixation time for the two groups did not have a statistical difference in the emotion recognition subtest, we can suggest that children with ASD have a look pattern that we call \"diffuse\", while children with TD have a look pattern which we call \"focused\" on regions of the face relevant to the recognition of emotions. It seems that in the ToM task, the fixation time in the eyes and mouth was not the determinant for the error in the response, but the diffuse look in the scene that was to be interpreted. When we evaluate the qualitative results of eye tracking we can think of the importance of interventions that can help children with ASD improve their performance in identifying the facial expressions that aid in the decoding of emotion and the interpretation of mental states

Page generated in 0.0492 seconds