Spelling suggestions: "subject:"bookreading"" "subject:"bookandreading""
1 |
The Efficacy of a Professional Development Program to Enhance Preschool Educators’ Ability to Facilitate Conversation During Shared Book ReadingMilburn, Trelani Faith 14 December 2011 (has links)
This study investigated the effects of a shared book reading professional development program on preschool educators’ ability to engage children in book-related conversations and promote word learning. 20 preschool educators were video-recorded reading two books to a small group of children at pre- and posttest eight weeks apart. Educators in the experimental group (n = 10) participated in professional development that included classroom instruction and individualized coaching. The control group remained on the waitlist. All video-recordings were transcribed and coded. Results indicated that educators in the experimental group included significantly more questions, responsive statements, and lexical diversity in their book-related talk compared to the control group. Further, they facilitated longer book-related conversations and had more long conversations (i.e., five turns or longer). Finally, conversations that included inferential talk resulted in the longest conversations. These findings suggest that professional development can enhance educators’ responsiveness during shared book reading.
|
2 |
The Efficacy of a Professional Development Program to Enhance Preschool Educators’ Ability to Facilitate Conversation During Shared Book ReadingMilburn, Trelani Faith 14 December 2011 (has links)
This study investigated the effects of a shared book reading professional development program on preschool educators’ ability to engage children in book-related conversations and promote word learning. 20 preschool educators were video-recorded reading two books to a small group of children at pre- and posttest eight weeks apart. Educators in the experimental group (n = 10) participated in professional development that included classroom instruction and individualized coaching. The control group remained on the waitlist. All video-recordings were transcribed and coded. Results indicated that educators in the experimental group included significantly more questions, responsive statements, and lexical diversity in their book-related talk compared to the control group. Further, they facilitated longer book-related conversations and had more long conversations (i.e., five turns or longer). Finally, conversations that included inferential talk resulted in the longest conversations. These findings suggest that professional development can enhance educators’ responsiveness during shared book reading.
|
3 |
Parent Goals and Beliefs and their Prediction of Behaviour during Shared Book ReadingAudet, Diana Rose 09 May 2013 (has links)
In this dissertation two studies are reported to explore the relation between parents’ goals for, beliefs about, and behaviour during parent-child shared book reading. In the first study, 92 parents rated the importance of potential reasons for reading with their children using the Parent Goals for Shared Reading Questionnaire (Evans & Williams, 2003). Ratings were completed longitudinally each year from their child’s junior kindergarten to grade 1 year. Factor analysis largely confirmed previous cross-sectional findings that the questionnaire items reflect distinct parent goals for shared book reading, including fostering reading skills, stimulating development, engaging in an enjoyable activity, and experiencing closeness with their child. The second study used data from the senior kindergarten year of this same sample of children to investigate the
relation between parents’ beliefs about how to teach reading, how highly they rated fostering reading skills as a goal versus non-reading goal subsets, and their behaviour during shared book reading (i.e., the nature of the extratextual comments and error corrections made). Results demonstrated that, as parent goals to foster children’s reading skills increase, their tendency to provide the correct word in response to their child’s miscues decreases. When goals to foster reading skills were high, parents were more likely to use strategies that help their child to read misread words, regardless of their ratings of goals to engage in a positive experience with their child. However, when goals to foster reading skills were low, increasing ratings of the goal to engage in a positive experience related to decreasing instances of parents helping their child to read misread words. Furthermore, the goal to engage in a positive experience with one’s child was positively related to extratextual conversation not related to teaching reading but only when goals to foster reading skills were also low. Finally, goals to foster reading and constructivist beliefs both made independent contributions to the prediction of parents’ use of context cues to correct reading errors. Implications of these findings for literacy intervention programs targeted at the shared-book reading interaction are discussed.
|
4 |
Vocabulary Acquisition: An Investigation of Prompted and Spontaneous Vocabulary Use in Preschool Children during Dialogic Book ReadingHedges, Erin M. 22 August 2008 (has links)
No description available.
|
5 |
USING BOOKS TO IMPROVE MENTAL ROTATION SKILLS WITH 4- AND 5-YEAR-OLD CHILDRENTavassolie, Nadia 05 1900 (has links)
Mental rotation skills predict later achievement in STEM (Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009). Prior research shows that children’s mental rotation skills improve after training (Hawes, Gilligan-Lee, & Mix, 2022; Uttal et al., 2013). However, most studies have used dynamic stimuli where children see objects rotating. We hypothesized that reading books that practice mental rotation with only static images could improve children’ mental rotation skills. We preregistered a pretest-training-posttest design with 4- and 5-year-olds (N = 80). Children completed a mental rotation assessment at pretest, four to six reading days with an experimenter over two weeks in one of two randomly-assigned conditions (Mental Rotation Book Condition versus Control Book Condition), and a mental rotation assessment at posttest. The Mental Rotation Books involved mental rotation practice while the Control Books did not.
Consistent with our hypothesis, condition was a significant predictor of posttest mental rotation accuracy, controlling for age, verbal ability, and pretest mental rotation accuracy. Children in the Mental Rotation Book condition significantly improved from pretest (M = .59, SD = .24) to posttest (M = .75, SD = .21), while the control group did not. However, condition was not a significant predictor of posttest mental transformation skills, math achievement, or spatial vocabulary, controlling for age, verbal ability, and respective pretest scores.
Book-reading may be a scalable method for improving mental rotation skills in early childhood and warrants further intervention studies using book-reading at home or in schools to improve spatial skills. / Psychology
|
6 |
Parent Perspectives of a Dialogic Book Reading WorkshopSlocum, Camille 10 June 2024 (has links) (PDF)
The purpose of this study is to identify how parents perceive dialogic book reading workshops that they participate in, and how cultural backgrounds affect these perspectives. Four native English-speaking mothers, and one Spanish-speaking mother with preschool-age children participated in this study. After two dialogic book reading workshops, parents participated in focus groups to discuss their perceptions. Participants noted three positive themes including motivators like incentives and childcare, positive influences, and effective adaptations in their reading routines with their children, and how the workshops were structured with helpful facilitators, supplemental materials, and content. Participants suggested various ideas for improvement. The Spanish-speaking participant mentioned more familial benefits, while the English-speaking participants focused on individual benefits. Overall, parents perceive dialogic book reading workshops as positive experiences that positively impact their families across cultures.
|
7 |
Language Development in Preschoolers at Risk: Linguistic Input among Head Start Parents and Oral Narrative Performance of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing ChildrenGoldberg, Hanah 08 January 2016 (has links)
The development of children’s language skills during the preschool years plays a crucial role in subsequent reading and school success. Some children may enter kindergarten with oral language skills that lag behind their peers’. Two such groups are children from low-socioeconomic status (SES) families and those who are deaf or hard of hearing (DHH).
Study 1 considered parents’ linguistic input during interactions with their Head Start pre-kindergarten children in two conversational contexts. The first, shared storybook reading, has featured prominently in early language interventions but proven less efficacious among low-SES samples. The second, shared reminiscing, offers a theoretically promising setting in which to promote child vocabulary skills but lacks empirical support. This study examined features of parental language known to relate to children’s vocabulary, including parents’ quantity of speech, lexical diversity, syntactic complexity, and intent to elicit child language. Parents’ and children’s expressive vocabulary knowledge was also considered. Forty parent-child dyads’ conversations during storybook reading and shared reminiscing were audiorecorded, transcribed, analyzed, and coded. Paired t-tests revealed that, while parents talked more during book reading, they used greater levels of syntactic complexity and language-eliciting talk during shared reminiscing. Parents’ own vocabulary knowledge was related to their children’s but not to linguistic input in either context.
Study 2 considered the oral narrative skills of DHH preschoolers relative to language-matched hearing children. School-age DHH children often experience delays in the development of narrative skills compared to their hearing peers. Little is known about the narrative abilities of DHH children during the preschool years. This study examined 46 DHH and 58 vocabulary-matched hearing preschoolers’ overall language production, lexical diversity, syntactic complexity, and narrative comprehension skills. DHH children produced a similar number of words and demonstrated similar levels of narrative understanding compared to their hearing peers. However, DHH children’s narratives contained significantly less complex syntax. Gains in lexical diversity differed by group, with DHH children demonstrating less growth over the course of the school year despite making more gains on a standardized measure of vocabulary.
Implications for instruction, assessment, and future research are discussed for both low-SES and DHH children.
|
8 |
Improving Head Start Teachers' Concept Development: Long Term Follow-Up of a Training Program and Differences in Program ImpactLipp, Amanda KR 01 April 2016 (has links)
Children from a low socioeconomic status (SES) home environment are typically exposed to less vocabulary during the first few years of life and experience higher rates of poor school readiness, particularly in emergent literacy skills, when compared to middle-class peers (Bowey, 1995; Hart & Risley, 2003; Whitehurst, 1997). Early childhood education programs designed to expose this group to cognitively challenging utterances have found that low SES children tend to make greater gains in vocabulary development compared to middle-class peers (Justice, Meier, & Walpole, 2005).
|
9 |
Comment des adultes et des enfants, âgés de 3 à 6 ans, racontent ensemble des histoires en situations familiale et scolaire / How adults and children, aged from 3 to 6, tell stories together at home and at schoolVinel, Elise 09 December 2014 (has links)
Cette recherche s’inscrit dans une approche dialogique et interactionniste du langage. Dans la suite des travaux soulignant l’importance de la culture (Tomasello, 2004) et des récits (Bruner, 1983, 1996, 2006), ainsi que des interactions qui y ont lieu mais aussi dans ceux analysant les interactions ayant lieu plus particulièrement au cours de lectures conjointes d’histoires, ou encore de ceux de Bakhtine (1984), Brès (1994), François (2004) ou encore Labov (1978) analysant le fonctionnement des discours et plus particulièrement des récits, nous cherchons à comprendre comment adultes et enfants racontent ensemble des histoires en situation familiale et scolaire. Dix enfants âgés de 3 à 5 ans ont été filmés au cours de ces séances de lectures d’albums, sans texte et avec texte, avec un de leur parent à leur domicile et avec des pairs et des enseignants à l’école. Les analyses portent sur la manière dont les adultes s’y prennent pour non seulement faire participer les enfants à la production de récits, mais aussi co-construire avec eux les récits ou encore co-construire avec eux la référence. Les résultats montrent malgré un certain nombre d’invariants une grande diversité des conduites narratives des adultes et des enfants. / This research revolves around both a dialogical and an interactionist approach of language. This study follows at once previous researches highlighting the importance of culture (Tomasello, 2004), interactions and narratives (Bruner, 1983, 1996, 2006), as well as researches studying interactions during joint activities of book-reading, and studies analysing the way discourse, and especially narratives work (Bakhtine, 1984, Brès 1994, François, 2004 ou encore Labov, 1978). We aim at understanding how adults and children tell stories together, at home and at school. Ten children aged from three to five have been recorded during book-reading sessions, with and without text, at home with one parent and at school with teachers and other children. We have carried out analyses on the way adults do not only to make the children participate to the narratives’ production, but also co-construct narratives or reference with the children. Despite a certain amount of invariants, the results show a wide variety of narrative discursive behaviors among adults and children.
|
10 |
Environmental, behavioural, and cognitive predictors of emergent literacy and reading skillsStephenson, Kathy 06 1900 (has links)
This thesis consists of three separate papers broadly examining how different environmental and child variables affect language and literacy acquisition in two or more orthographies. The first paper is a quantitative meta-analysis of studies that have examined the effects of shared book reading on language, emergent literacy skills, and reading achievement with preschool children. The results suggest that shared book reading explained approximately 7% of variance in all the language and literacy measures combined. The mean effect size of shared book reading was slightly larger for the combined language measures (d = 0.77) than for the combined emergent literacy measures (d = 0.57), or the combined reading achievement measures (d = 0.63). An examination of the effects of shared book reading on specific language, emergent literacy, and reading skills revealed that shared book reading is more related to some skills than others.
The second paper examines the effects of home literacy (shared book reading, teaching activities, and number of books), children’s task-focused behaviour, and parents’ beliefs and expectations about their child’s reading and academic ability on Kindergarten children’s (N = 61) phonological sensitivity and letter knowledge and on Grade 1 word reading. The results showed that after controlling for nonverbal IQ and vocabulary, parent teaching activities prior to Kindergarten predicted significantly letter knowledge; parents’ beliefs about their children’s reading ability predicted significantly phonological sensitivity and Kindergarten word reading; and children’s task-focused behaviour predicted significantly letter knowledge and Kindergarten and Grade 1 word reading
The third paper reports on a cross-linguistic longitudinal study that examines the environmental, behavioural, and cognitive predictors of Grade 3 word reading fluency, passage comprehension, and spelling in children learning to read in an orthographically inconsistent language (English) and in an orthographically consistent language (Greek). Results indicated that home literacy factors did not directly predict Grade 3 reading or spelling skills for either the English- or Greek-speaking samples. Task-focused behaviour directly predicted spelling for the Greek-speaking sample. Vocabulary was more important for reading and spelling in English than in Greek. Letter knowledge was more important for spelling in Greek and for passage comprehension in English. / Special Education
|
Page generated in 0.0575 seconds