1 |
Antipathies and attribution:the effects on self-esteem, self-efficacy, and task persistenceSaxon, Caroline Ruth 30 April 2011 (has links)
This study looked at how antipathies and the attributions made to our enemies concerning the outcomes of important life events affect one‟s self-esteem, self-efficacy, and task persistence. The results did not support either of the two hypotheses studied. However, it was found that those who succeeded persisted longer on the provided task when attributing the success to their enemy. This could be due to participants believing that they could show up the antipathy, giving them someone to compete against, which in turn lead to higher task persistence. Also, it was found that those who made external attributions while involved with an antipathy got more items correct on the task provided. Making external attributions for events could lead to a desire to prove oneself and in turn increase task persistence.
|
2 |
The Effect of Ability-Based Verses Effort-Based Praise on Task Performance and Persistence for Children with GiftednessFisher, Adria 01 April 2009 (has links)
No description available.
|
3 |
The Effect of Resilience on Task Performance and Persistence during Repeated Exposure to Heat PainSlepian, Peter M. January 2015 (has links)
No description available.
|
4 |
The Restorative Effects of Smoking upon Self-Control ResourcesHeckman, Bryan W. 09 September 2010 (has links)
Based on a model that considers self-control (SC) to be a limited resource, research
suggests that diminished SC resources increase the likelihood of smoking. Yet, no study
has evaluated how smoking affects SC resources. This study used a randomized, 2 x 2
crossed factorial (SC depletion manipulation X smoking manipulation), between-subjects
design to test the hypothesis that smoking restores depleted SC resources. To manipulate
SC depletion, half of the 132 dependent smokers were instructed to suppress their
emotional reaction to a brief video depicting environmental damage (i.e., Depletion),
whereas the other half were instructed to “act natural” (i.e., No Depletion) during
viewing. Half of the participants in each condition then smoked a cigarette, whereas the
other half sat patiently, without smoking (i.e., Smoke vs. No Smoke). All participants
then completed two behavioral measures of SC (Mirror Tracing Performance Task:
MTPT; and breath-holding). As hypothesized, a disordinal interaction occurred between
the Depletion and Smoking manipulations for duration of time spent on the MTPT. That
is, participants in the depletion condition showed less persistence on the MTPT, unless
they were permitted to smoke. There was no evidence for mediation of this effect from
the influence of smoking on affect and/or urge. Thus, smoking appeared to restore
depleted SC resources, independent of its effects on self-reported affect and urge.
Findings suggest that restoration of SC resources may represent another form of negative
reinforcement from smoking that may play a role in nicotine dependence, and could
inform treatment development.
|
5 |
Task Persistence in Early Childhood EducationStephenson, Thomas Richard 01 May 1973 (has links)
This study was designed to discover developmental trends in task persistence (TP). It was hoped that the results would suggest when and for what groups a task persistence (TP) curriculum is crucial.
To study this development a sample of two, four, and six year old children were given a "work" task. The task required 120 subjects (Ss) to sort and fold 33 items of clothing and linen. After the task instructions and demonstration, the experimenter did not intervene with reinforcers or further directions.
The dependent variables investigated were time spent on the task, time spent away from the task, and number of tasks completed. The independent variables were age, race (Black and White), sex, and social class (advantaged and disadvantaged). The data were analyzed with analysis of variance, Pearson's Product-Moment Correlation, and chi square.
The results indicated that older children spent significantly more time on task and significantly less time away from task than younger children. Black children spent significantly more time (than White children) on and away from the task at all ages. Advantaged children spent significantly more time (than disadvantaged children) on task at all ages and significantly less time away from task at ages two and four. Advantaged and disadvantaged subjects were not differentiated by the time spent away from the task at age six. These results imply that task persistence training programs could begin as early as age two. They also suggest that certain subgroups (i.e., disadvantaged White) may require more intensive task persistence training then other subgroups (i.e., advantaged Black).
Completion of tasks (CT) significantly differentiated six year olds from two and four year olds, but failed to differentiate two and four year olds. Tasks were completed significantly more often at ages two and four by males than females and by advantaged than disadvantaged children. However, these differences disappeared by age six. Since the differences faded at age six, the use of completion of tasks training as a general curriculum goal may not be warranted.
Some of the most surprising results were described by the significant correlation coefficients. The total time spent on the task was negatively related to completion of task. Completion of task was positively related to the time spent away from the task and number of times away from task. These findings suggest that brief periods of absence from a task may improve the chances of completing the task. Therefore, optimal "work" performance for early childhood education may be realized by programming frequent, brief "breaks."
A secondary purpose of the study was to investigate verbalizations expressed during the experimental sessions. To study this variable, a verbatim record was established and maintained for each subject. The verbalizations were then totaled and classified as task relevant or irrelevant. The results were analyzed with an analysis of variance (age x race x sex x social class) and Pearson's Product Moment Correlation. Although the number of verbalizations increased with age, the differences between ages four and six were not significant. This finding may reflect a shift to covert verbal mediation during "work" performance on or about age four.
The developmental patterns of verbalizations were different for boys and girls. The girls used more overt verbalizations (than boys) at ages two and four; however, at age six the girls' number of verbalizations decreased noticeably. The verbalizations of boys increased with age. At age six, they used more verbalizations than the girls. The writer concluded that this finding reflected advanced verbalization skills in the girls. If the conclusion is valid, the results may mean that overt verbalizations are more necessary for boys to organize their experiences.
White children used significantly more task relevant verbalizations than Black children. This suggests that early childhood educational programs for predominately Black populations may need to stress task relevant verbalization training more than programs which serve predominately White populations.
Disadvantaged children used more task irrelevant verbalizations at ages two and six than advantaged children. Apparently, they need extra emphasis on task oriented verbalization training. When considered with the previous paragraph, the disadvantaged Black population has the greatest need for task relevant verbalization training.
|
6 |
The Effect Of Ability-Based Verse Effort-Based Praise On Task Performance And Persistence For Children With GiftednessSchmidt, Jessica Lee 09 November 2012 (has links)
No description available.
|
7 |
Behavioral and academic effects of brainologyTodd, Melissa Kay 04 November 2013 (has links)
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of a computer program for students with emotional behavioral disorder (EBD) on behavior and academics. This concurrent, multiple baseline study investigated the use of Brainology with three upper elementary students diagnosed with EBD. Evaluations across behavior, academics, and behavioral academic indicators resulted in indications of possible effectiveness with one fourth grade student and limited to no effects with the other two students. There was a lack of multiple demonstration of intervention effect in this study across the baseline for behavior.
Overall, student effort appeared to increase using this intervention, which is an important finding given the problems with disengagement that students with EBD experience (Wagner et al., 2004. Despite the limited results of this study, Brainology appears to hold some promise for students with EBD and it is hoped that further research will explore this possibility further. The teachers and students indicated that the treatment had strong to moderate validity on validity measures. Implications for Brainology and students with EBD are presented. Study limitations and directions for future research and practice are discussed. / text
|
8 |
The Influence of Personality Traits and Individual Beliefs on Task PersistenceElliott, Alexis Shae 19 January 2023 (has links)
No description available.
|
9 |
"The effect of ability-based versus effort-based praise on task performance, task persistence, and internal factors in children identified as gifted or talented in mathematics"Greene, Robert W. 09 April 2014 (has links)
No description available.
|
Page generated in 0.098 seconds