Spelling suggestions: "subject:"cognitive psychology"" "subject:"aognitive psychology""
911 |
Effects of elaborations in expository texts: Large time cost, reduced attentionand lower memory for main ideasDaley, Nola M. 01 July 2019 (has links)
No description available.
|
912 |
Systems Factorial Technology extended to bilateral visual fields and model predictions testingFan, Gaojie, Fan 07 January 2020 (has links)
No description available.
|
913 |
The Capacity of Visual Working Memory During Visual SearchKing, Michael J. 01 June 2020 (has links)
No description available.
|
914 |
An Investigation of the Role of Contrast Cues in Parainformative CategorizationWimsatt, Jay A., Jr. 28 September 2020 (has links)
No description available.
|
915 |
Moral Disengagement of Violent and Nonviolent Antisocial Behavior in Video GamesBailey, Michael Hampton January 2020 (has links)
No description available.
|
916 |
Examining the Bilingual Advantage in Visuospatial Executive Function Tasks for Regular Use BilingualsJensen, Jessica A. January 2020 (has links)
No description available.
|
917 |
Using Process Tracing and Computational Modeling to Investigate Cognition During Risky Decision MakingPettit, Elizabeth Jean 22 April 2021 (has links)
No description available.
|
918 |
The Neurobehavioral Basis of the Parallel Individuation (PI) and Approximation Number System (ANS)Tang, Jean Ee January 2023 (has links)
Research on numerical cognition proposes that there are two systems for the perception of numerical quantity, a small-number system (1~3) invoking parallel individuation, or “subitizing”, and a large-number system (4+) that is based on Weberian magnitude estimation (Hyde, 2011). Many numerical cognitive neuroscientists have focused on studying how the magnitude of numerosities (small vs. large numbers) and numerical distance (close vs. far differences between numbers) are influential factors when processing numbers and change detection. However, is there a difference when numerosities are increasing or decreasing? The effects of direction on numerical change processing are lesser known.
This 128-channel EEG study investigated the neurobehavioral basis of differentiation between small vs. large-number perception and effects of change directionality. During EEG data collection, participants were sequentially presented with stimulus arrays of 1 to 6 dots, with parameters like size and location controlled for, to minimize varying non-numerical visual cues during habituation. Participants were instructed to press a key whenever they detect a change in the number of dots presented.
The current study adapts a dot-stimuli numerical change study design from Hyde and Spelke (2009, 2012). In their EEG study, the researchers examined event-related-potential (ERP) differences during the processing of small (1, 2, 3) and large (8, 16, 24) numbers. For this study, we chose to examine a narrower numerical range from 1~6, so that small (1, 2, 3) vs. large (4, 5, 6) contrasts were along a numerical continuum. In contrast to Hyde and Spelke (2009, 2012), where participants passively-viewed the sequential presentation of dot arrays, this study employed an active change detection paradigm, where participants’ reaction time (RT) and accuracy in detecting change in the number of dots were recorded.
We investigated the effects of Direction and Size in numerical change detection, where Direction is operationally defined as Decreasing and Increasing change in numeric set size, while Size is divided into Small-to-Small, Large-to-Large and Crossovers. Numerical change conditions were categorized into six groups: “Increasing Small-to-Small” (e.g., 1-to-2, 2-to-3), “Decreasing Small-to-Small” (e.g., 2-to-1, 3-to-2), “Increasing Large-Large” (e.g., 4-to-6, 5-to-6), “Decreasing Large-Large” (e.g., 5-to-4, 6-to-5), Increasing Small-to-Large” (e.g., 2-to-4, 3-to-5, 3-to-6) and “Decreasing Large-to-Small” (e.g., 4-to-2, 5-to-2, 6-to-3), where the last two groups are operationally defined as Crossovers. There was also a “No Change” condition, where the number of dots remain the same for up to five presentations. ERP analyses were conducted for the N1 component (125-200 ms) over the left and right occipital-temporal-parietal (POT) junction and for the P3b component (435-535 ms) over the midline parietal area (Pz).
During the No Change condition, results show that the N1 amplitude was modulated by the cardinal values of the habituated numbers 1~6. Within this continuous range, we found N1 amplitudes commensurate with cardinal values in the small range (1, 2, 3), but not in the large range (4, 5, 6), suggesting that numbers in the subitizing range are individuated as objects in working memory.
Meanwhile, in the Change condition, there was a significant main effect of Direction on N1 peak latency, where the Increasing condition showed earlier peaks. In the Decreasing Small-to-Small condition, N1 amplitudes were the lowest (even lower than N1 peaks for No Change conditions), while the other five Change conditions all produced higher N1 negativities than No Change conditions. These results imply that when the number of dots get small enough to parallel individuate, instead of encoding items into visual short-term memory, the brain is “off-loading” items from our perceptual load.
Intriguingly, although the Decreasing Small-to-Small condition had the lowest N1 negativities, it produced the highest P3b positivity. Distinctions in P3b waveforms reflect a clear categorical break between small vs. large numbers, where easier/small number change conditions have higher amplitudes than harder, large number conditions, suggesting more difficulty with updating the context in the latter. However, in contrast to the earlier N1, there was no main effect of Direction on P3b peak latency, but there was an interaction effect of Direction by Size.
Interestingly, there was also a similar interaction effect of Direction by Size for reaction times, with similar trends showing that Decreasing conditions produced shorter reaction times for the Large-to-Large and Crossover conditions, yet this pattern was reversed in the Small-to-Small condition. This lends more support to the implication of the “off-loading” phenomenon when processing decreases of numerosities in the small range (1~3). Meanwhile, when it comes to context-updating at later stages, and a behavioral response is required for this change detection task, the Large-to-Large condition prove to be the most difficult, as there was lower accuracy, longer reaction times, later and lower P3b peaks.
N1 and P3b amplitudes are complementary to each other, with the early N1 being more sensitive to Direction, and the later P3b being more sensitive to Size. This suggests that the posterior parietal cortex might encode Direction first, followed by Size. This study proposes a model that is an adaptation to the P3b context-updating model (Donchin, 1981), where the early, sensory N1 interplays with the later, cognitive P3b. These findings suggest a neurobehavioral basis for the differentiation of small vs. large number perception at early stages of processing that is sensitive to encoding vs. off-loading objects from perceptual load and visual short-term memory, as well as a later stage that involve higher-order cognitive processing on the magnitude of set size that is employed in numerical change detection tasks.
|
919 |
Hindsight for foresight: adaptive uses of memory in value-based decision makingNicholas, Jonathan January 2023 (has links)
Effective decision making depends on using memories of past experiences to inform choices in the present. This dissertation examines several ways in which memory is used in decision making, and further aims to establish that one way we adapt to the statistics of our environment is by modifying how we use our memories to guide behavior. In chapters one and two, I focus on how incremental trial-and-error learning and episodic memories of individual events may each contribute to choice.
In chapter one, I ask how the brain may arbitrate rationally between these two systems to achieve a balance that maximizes reward. By manipulating the volatility of the environment to affect uncertainty, I show that participants rely on each system in the circumstances to which it is best suited.
In chapter two, I then ask how decisions based on these memory systems each depend on striatal dopamine. By studying patients with Parkinson’s disease both on and off their medication, I find that a lack of dopamine alters only incremental learning, and that dopamine replacement remediates this deficit with few effects on the use of episodic memory.
Finally, in chapter three, I examine a more difficult class of decisions that require individual memories to be used for planning future action. Using neuroimaging to decode memory access, I find that that the statistical structure of relationships between memories determines when they are used to support planning. Combined, these three chapters suggest that we are capable of flexibly employing multiple forms of memory, with distinct neural mechanisms, to guide a variety of choices.
|
920 |
Examining the Effects of a High Fat, High Sugar Diet in Adolescence on Memory and Executive Functioning in Young AdulthoodMurray, Susan January 2021 (has links)
Accumulating evidence from animal studies suggests that diets high in fats and sugar lead to poorer cognitive functioning. Importantly, animals exposed to a high fat, high sugar (HFHS) diet during adolescence show more pronounced deficits in cognitive performance than animals given this diet during adulthood, suggesting an age-specific vulnerability for diet-induced cognitive impairments. Given that the three primary sources of daily caloric intake for children and adolescents in the United States are dessert, pizza, and soda, translational research is needed to better understand the link between diet during development and cognitive function. Some studies demonstrate long-term effects of adolescent exposure to HFHS diets, highlighting a need for longitudinal research in this area.
The current study sought to investigate whether unhealthy dietary habits during adolescent development predicts performance on tasks of memory and executive function using publicly available data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health study). Using three separate linear regressions, we tested whether HFHS intake in adolescence predicts memory and executive function in young adulthood using the following outcomes as dependent variables: total word recall score (immediate trial), total word recall score (delayed trial), and total number recall score. We also tested whether a robust indicator of inflammation, high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP), which was measured in adulthood, mediates the relationship between HFHS intake in adolescence and cognitive performance in adulthood. Finally, we tested whether physical activity in adolescence moderates the relationship between HFHS intake in adolescence and hsCRP as well as cognitive performance in adulthood.
The results of the regression analyses reveal that HFHS scores in adolescence significantly and negatively predict performance on both the immediate and delayed word recall trials in adulthood, even after controlling for relevant covariates such as SES and BMI. The effect of HFHS scores on number recall scores was trending toward significance. The relationship between diet and memory was mediated by hsCRP, though HFHS scores was negatively associated with hsCRP. Physical activity did not moderate the effect of diet on hsCRP or cognitive performance.
These findings support animal and human studies showing a relationship between HFHS intake and poorer cognitive performance. Importantly, the results of the current study extend the existing literature by suggesting that HFHS intake during adolescent development may affect cognitive performance later in life. Replication of this study is needed along with further research to identify possible physiological mechanisms underlying the relationship between HFHS and cognition as well as factors that modify this relationship. / Psychology
|
Page generated in 0.0829 seconds