Spelling suggestions: "subject:"stuttering"" "subject:"stutterings""
91 |
A Study of the Duration of Words Surrounding a Moment of StutteringPeterson, Jennifer Dawn 05 May 1995 (has links)
Until this point, not much research has examined the difference in temporal characteristics for untreated stutterers in words surrounding a moment of stuttering. It is important to determine whether or not stutterers who have not been in treatment alter the duration of their speech when they stutter versus when they are fluent to determine what aids in the increase of fluency. The purpose of the present study was to examine the duration of the word prior to and following a stuttered word and the duration of the corresponding word in a nonstuttered episode. The following questions were to be addressed: 1) Is there a significant durational difference between a word preceding a stuttered word and the duration of the same word in a corresponding nonstuttered sentence? 2) Is there a significant difference between a word following a stuttered word and the duration of the same word in a corresponding nonstuttered sentence? Three subjects age 16 and older who had not received treatment for at least 4 years were selected. Subjects were recorded reading a list of 83 sentences selected from Fairbanks (1940) twice. Sentences containing a word that was stuttered in one reading and not in the other were used for analysis. The duration of the following word pairs in milliseconds was computed via the CSRE 4.2 program (Jaimeson, D.G., Ramji, K.V., Neary, T., & Baxter, T., 1993): 1) The duration of the word preceding a stuttered word within the same sentence (BSTUT) . 2) The duration of the same word in the corresponding nonstuttered sentence (BNSTUT) . 3) The duration of the word following a stuttered word within the same sentence (ASTUT) . 4) The duration of that same word in the corresponding nonstuttered sentence (ANSTUT) . A total of 144 samples were obtained. A two-tailed ~ test was run at the .05 level of confidence to determine significance between the BSTUT/BNSTUT and ASTUT/ANSTUT word pairs. Results yielded a significant difference between the durations of BSTUT and BNSTUT (P=.017). Conversely, analysis of the difference between ASTUT and ANSTUT revealed no significant difference in durations (P=.47).
|
92 |
Using time-out to treat advanced stutteringFranklin, Diane E. January 2002 (has links)
This study involved trialing an operant conditioning procedure known as time-out, as a treatment for adolescents and adults who stutter. Time-out requires individuals to pause briefly after stuttering and to resume talking after a pause in this case, of five seconds. A randomised control group design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of time-out treatment. Sixty participants were randomly assigned to either a time-out treatment or control group. The results demonstrated that individuals who stutter are highly responsive to time-out treatment. The impact of the severity of the stutter, a person's age, previous treatment, and the nature of the stutter on treatment outcome, were also investigated. Baseline severity was a strong predictor of treatment outcome, and to a lesser degree, previous treatment and speech rate were found to share some influence over treatment success. In addition, there was an unexpected change in the stuttering topography over the experiment conditions.
|
93 |
The effects of self-disclosure on listener perceptions of male and female individuals who stutterCappellini, Colleen Heather, 1985- 25 June 2012 (has links)
Research has shown that when a person who stutters self-discloses to a listener that he or she is a stutterer, this self-disclosure may positively impact the listeners' perceptions of the stuttering speaker. However, findings from previous studies have been limited in several ways. The purpose of this study was to further examine if listeners' perceptions of a stuttering speaker vary depending on whether or not the speaker self-discloses that they stutter, and if listener perceptions are subject to gender bias. We addressed limitations of prior studies by utilizing both male and female stuttering speakers who spoke directly to the viewer of the video, by balancing combinations of video viewings to account for potential effects of order, and by attempting to recruit a larger number of subjects. Participants (n =27) were randomly assigned to view two of the four possible videos (male self-disclosure, male no self-disclosure, female self-disclosure, and female no self-disclosure). After viewing both videos, participants immediately filled out a survey assessing their perceptions of the speakers' personality traits. Results for effects of self-disclosure achieved significance for "no difference" for traits of more intelligent, more unintelligent, and more unfriendly. These non-significant trends suggest self-disclosure might positively affect listener perceptions of a stuttering speaker, Results for effects of gender achieved significance for "no difference" for traits of more intelligent, more unintelligent, and more confident. In summary, results from this study show potential emerging trends that self-disclosure positively affects listeners' perceptions of stuttering speakers. / text
|
94 |
Support groups for people who stutter : the role and perspective of speech-language pathologistsDunaway, Laura Susan 21 November 2013 (has links)
In an effort to improve understanding of the role that support groups such as the National Stuttering Association (NSA) play in helping people who stutter, speech-language pathologists who participate in the NSA were asked to participate in a research survey. Previous research and anecdotal evidence has shown that support groups not only benefit people who stutter, but also the professionals who work with them. Participation in the NSA can enhance SLPs' understanding of stuttering, and their comfort level and competence working with people who stutter. However, the relationship between support groups and professionals has not been adequately studied. / text
|
95 |
The influence of stuttering awareness on the child who stutters' friendship preferenceJolly, Angela Marie 30 November 2010 (has links)
Stuttering has been defined as an atypical disruption in the forward flow of speech (Conture, 2001). The onset of stuttering is reported to be 2 to 3 years of age; the age at which the child is first learning to communicate more frequently with words rather than nonverbal behaviors. Interestingly, this is also the time at which children seek interactions with others rather than the former tendency to play independently. Because of the overt nature of this disorder, the timing of the onset and its co-occurrence with significant social developmental shifts, stuttering has the potential to impact the child’s ability to make and maintain friendships. The purpose of this report is to investigate the impact of stuttering awareness on the friendship preferences of preschool children who stutter. / text
|
96 |
Central auditory processing perforance of male and female stutterers and nonstutterers.Kathard, Harsha. January 1992 (has links)
Central auditory processing performance of male and female stutterers
and nonstutterers was compared on a battery of central
auditory tests. Thirty stutterers (15 male and 15 female) with a
mean age of 23.10 years (17.2-31 years) comprised the experimental
group, and 30 nonstutterers (15 male and 15 female) with a
mean age of 22.2 years(17-32 years) comprised the control group.
The test battery included dichotic (DCV test, ssw test, eST) and
monotic (SSI-ICM test, ARLT) tests. Stutterers performed significantly
poorer than nonstutterers on various parameters of individual
tests. The stutterers' performance on the test battery was
varied : 8(26.6%) stutterers passed all tests in the battery;
7(23.3%) failed dichotic tests only; 15(50%) failed dichotic and
monotic tests of which 2(6.6%) failed monotic tests. Pass/fail
rates indicated that although 15 (50%) nonstutterers failed the
battery 22(73.2%) stutterers failed. This result confirmed that
stutterers performed significantly differently from nonstutterers
on the test battery( X?= 19.87 , df=l; p<0.05). Male/famale comparisons
for nonstutterers indicated no significant differences
(p>0.05) on individual tests except on the ARLT where males
obtained longer latencies than females. Pass /fail rates on the
test battery confirmed no statistically significant (X~= 0.133 ,
df=l; p> 0.05) performance differences between male and female
nonstutterers. For stutterers, although male performance was
poorer than female performance on various parameters of individual
tests ,the performance differences were not significant
(p>0.05). However, pass/fail performance on the test battery
indicated that significantly more males (13) than females (9)
failed the test battery ( X2 = 8.66 df=l, p<0.05). The results
are discussed in terms of the literature and theoretical and
clinical implications are presented and discussed. / Thesis (M.Sc.)-University of Durban-Westville, 1992.
|
97 |
The value of creative drama in the treatment of stuttering.Chatrooghoon, Mawalall. January 1991 (has links)
No abstract available. / Thesis (Ph.D.)-University of Durban-Westville, 1991.
|
98 |
The relationship between salivary cortisol levels and self-perception of anxiety in adults who stutter across various speaking situationsDiehl, Janine January 2011 (has links)
Adults who stutter (AWS) are reported to have increased levels of anxiety compared to adults who do not stutter (AWNS), particularly in social interactions (Kraaimaat,
Vanryckeyham, & Dan-Baggen, 2002; Mahr & Torosian, 1999; Messenger, Onslow,
Packman, & Menzies, 2004). However, the level of perceived anxiety in AWS according to specific speaking situations has not been critically evaluated. In addition, most studies addressing state anxiety (i.e., communication apprehension) are based on self-judgments (Craig, 1990; Craig, Hancook, Tran, & Craig, 2003; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; Gabel, Colcord, & Petrosino, 2002; Lincoln, Onslow, & Menzies, 1996; Messenger et al., 2004; Miller & Watson, 1992; Mulcahy, Hennessey, Beilby & Byrnes, 2008) which have not been verified using a physiological evaluation of anxiety. The present study sought to examine the relationship between a physiological measure of anxiety (i.e., cortisol) and perceptual judgments of communication apprehension across different speaking situations. Ten AWS aged between 19-62 years, and ten sex- and aged-matched AWNS provided salivary cortisol samples during distinct speaking situations across a one-week period. The speaking situations consisted of (1) speaking face-to-face with a friend, (2) speaking face-to-face with a single stranger, (3) speaking in front of a group of four strangers, and (4) speaking to a stranger on the telephone. Each participant also provided self-perception assessments of their perceived anxiety levels using an adaptation of the Speaking Task Response Scale (STRS; Bray & James, 2009) before and after each speaking situation. Results of the cortisol analysis revealed no statistical difference in cortisol levels across the four speaking situations between AWS and AWNS. A significant difference was found between self-perceived anxiety levels in the pre-speaking situation between AWS and AWNS. Speaking face-to-face with a friend was perceived by the AWS to result in the lowest level of anxiety compared to the remaining three situations. Correlational analyses revealed a significant relationship between cortisol levels and self-reported anxiety in the AWS group but no such relationship was evident for AWNS. On the basis of the combined results from the cortisol and self-perception analyses it can be concluded that AWS differ from AWNS in their communication apprehension, most notably in regard to speaking in any situation other than a familiar person (e.g., friend). This difference between AWS and AWNS is most evident in measures of self-perception, although it is likely there is an associated physiological contributing factor.
|
99 |
Dichotic listening among adults who stutterLynn, Wanita L January 2010 (has links)
Dichotic listening of auditory stimuli is used to assess brain lateralisation by simultaneously presenting different stimuli to the left and right ears to determine which syllable was perceived as being the clearest. There is a limited, albeit dated number of studies that have examined dichotic listening performance in adults who stutter (AWS) and the results remain inconclusive. The aim of this research was to investigate whether AWS show a difference in the magnitude of the right ear advantage (REA) in both undirected and directed attentional tasks when compared with adults who do not stutter (AWNS). There were 14 right-handed participants, consisting of seven AWS and seven age and sex matched AWNS controls. All participants were screened for normal hearing. They completed a dichotic listening task, which included undirected and directed attentional listening tasks. Participants were to select the consonant-vowel (CV) pair they heard the clearest. The interaural intensity difference (IID) was modulated randomly during the undirected attention task. The results for the undirected task revealed: (1) a significant REA for AWS for the IID conditions of 0 to +21 dB and significant left ear advantages (LEA) for IIDs of -15 to -21 dB; (2) a significant REA for AWNS for the IID conditions of -9 to +21 dB and significant LEAs for IIDs of -18 to -21 dB; (3) laterality index scores with a significant IID effect but no significant group or group-by-ear interaction effects using parametric statistics. Further analysis of laterality using non-parametric statistics found significant differences between the fluency groups. In general, the findings in this study were revealing of differences between AWS and AWNS when performing dichotic listening tasks with speech stimuli. The primary difference observed between groups was in regards to the IID point at which a previous REA became a LEA. This “crossing-over” point occurred later for AWNS, indicating a strong left hemisphere advantage for the processing of speech. The earlier “crossing-over” for AWS would indicate that the right hemisphere was activated sooner for the processing of speech compared to AWNS. This activation of the right hemisphere is assumed to reflect more diffuse cerebral lateralisation for speech processing for the AWS and confirms past brain imaging studies. In the directed attention task, there was no significant difference between AWS and AWNS indicating that instances of stuttering may occur due to more automatic (bottom-up) speech processing. These findings have implications for theories of laterality and hemispheric asymmetry for phonological processing for AWS, which has been suggested to reflect a subgroup of AWS for whom cerebral dominance is related to their disfluency.
|
100 |
An investigation of syntactic skills and syntactic loci of disfluencies in stutterers, highly disfluent nonstutterers, and highly fluent nonstutterersTirsway, Deborah S. January 1976 (has links)
This study compared the syntactic skills of stutterers, highly disfluent nonstutterers, and highly fluent nonstutterers utilizing Developmental Sentence Scoring (DSS) procedures. Incorporating analysis of covariance techniques with age and speaker classification as independent variables, significant differences among total DSS scores were found for speaker classifications. Since mean DSS scores for stutterers fell between the mean scores of the two nonstuttering groups, no clear relationship between stuttering and syntactic skills was determined.Loci of disfluencies for stutterers were analyzed by individual categories within the DSS model utilizing one-way analysis of variance with repeated measures techniques. Loci of disfluencies occurred significantly more frequently in the categories of Wh-questions, conjunctions, and personal pronouns. Limitations of this research were given to account for these differences.
|
Page generated in 0.0999 seconds