• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 205
  • 45
  • 34
  • 29
  • 16
  • 11
  • 6
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 3
  • 3
  • 2
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 445
  • 194
  • 105
  • 104
  • 102
  • 92
  • 88
  • 76
  • 61
  • 60
  • 57
  • 46
  • 45
  • 38
  • 36
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
141

The learning of spelling among Hong Kong secondary students

Chow, Ka Po Winnie 01 January 2003 (has links)
No description available.
142

Words You Should Know How to Spell: An A to Z Guide to Perfect Spelling

Hatcher, David 18 August 2010 (has links)
Do you have trouble spelling everyday words? Is your spell check on overdrive? Well, this easy-to-use dictionary is just what you need! Organized with speed and convenience in mind, it gives you instant access to the correct spellings of more than 12,500 words. Also provided are quick tips and memory tricks, like: Help yourself get the spelling of their right by thinking of the phrase ?their heirlooms.? Most words ending in a ?seed? sound are spelled ?-cede? or ?-ceed,? but one word ends in ?-sede.? You could say the rule for spelling this word supersedes the other rules. No matter what you’re working on, you can be confident that your good writing won’t be marred by bad spelling. This book takes away the guesswork and helps you make a good impression! / https://dc.etsu.edu/alumni_books/1026/thumbnail.jpg
143

Non-orthographic consonant cluster manipulation by good and poor spellers /

Power, Luke, January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--Memorial University of Newfoundland, 2005. / Bibliography: leaves 78-81.
144

The Effects of Handwriting, Spelling, and T-Units on Holistic Scoring with Implications for Dysgraphia

Hooten, Regina 2009 December 1900 (has links)
This study examined the relationship of holistic scoring with handwriting legibility, spelling accuracy and number of T-units within compositions written by children in grades 3 through 6 using path analysis. A sample of 223 compositions was rated for handwriting legibility and composition quality, and coded for number of T-units and percentage of accurately spelled words. Number of T-units was consistently the strongest predictor of holistic scoring across the four grade levels. Handwriting legibility and spelling accuracy yielded varying results in different grade levels.
145

Metalinguistic Awareness Contributions: Evidence from Spelling in Korean and English

Yeon, Sookkyung 14 March 2013 (has links)
Metalinguistic awareness skills (i.e., phonological awareness, orthographic awareness, morphological awareness) contribute to children’s spelling as well as reading. Although the multidimensional nature of these metalinguistic awareness skills has been acknowledged, little research has been conducted on the simultaneous investigation of these three metalinguistic skills, and it is especially true for Korean Hangul. The purpose of this study was to simultaneously examine these three inter-related constructs and the unique and shared contributions of each construct to English spelling as well as Korean spelling of typically developing fourth, fifth and sixth grade Korean-speaking students (n= 287). Korean metalinguistic awareness skills represented by three-first order factors (i.e., phonological, orthographic, morphological awareness) predicted 83% of the total variance in Korean spelling, and 52% of the total variance in English Word Spelling. It was particularly noteworthy that Korean metalinguistic awareness skills determined 12% of the variance in English Word Spelling, even after controlling for English vocabulary, demonstrating that there was a transfer effect between the two different orthographies. Findings from the present study provide strong support for the relationships between first language and second language literacy skills in terms of spelling and the concrete relationship between morphological awareness and spelling.
146

The Effects of Handwriting, Spelling, and T-Units on Holistic Scoring with Implications for Dysgraphia

Hooten, Regina 2009 December 1900 (has links)
This study examined the relationship of holistic scoring with handwriting legibility, spelling accuracy and number of T-units within compositions written by children in grades 3 through 6 using path analysis. A sample of 223 compositions was rated for handwriting legibility and composition quality, and coded for number of T-units and percentage of accurately spelled words. Number of T-units was consistently the strongest predictor of holistic scoring across the four grade levels. Handwriting legibility and spelling accuracy yielded varying results in different grade levels.
147

Ανάπτυξη ορθογραφικών δεξιοτήτων σε παιδιά με και χωρίς μαθησιακές δυσκολίες

Γκαρώνη, Παρασκευή 13 July 2010 (has links)
Στόχος της παρούσας μελέτης ήταν η σύγκριση των ορθογραφικών επιδόσεων μαθητών της Δ΄ τάξης, της Ε΄ τάξης και της Στ΄ τάξης του δημοτικού σχολείου με μαθησιακές δυσκολίες με μαθητές χωρίς μαθησιακές δυσκολίες, των ίδιων τάξεων, στην ορθογραφική απόδοση λεξικών μορφημάτων λέξεων της ελληνικής γλώσσας. Στην έρευνα έλαβαν μέρος 927 παιδιά. Από αυτά 776 ήταν παιδιά χωρίς μαθησιακές δυσκολίες ενώ 151 ήταν παιδιά με μαθησιακές δυσκολίες. Τα αποτελέσματα της έρευνας, απόλυτα συμβατά με ευρήματα άλλων ερευνών, έδειξαν ότι οι επιδόσεις των παιδιών με μαθησιακές δυσκολίες ήταν, με στατιστικά σημαντική διαφορά, χαμηλότερες από τις επιδόσεις των παιδιών χωρίς δυσκολίες της ίδιας ηλικίας ενώ έδειξαν επίσης ότι η διαφορά παραμένει ακόμα και όταν συγκριθούν οι επιδόσεις των παιδιών με μαθησιακές δυσκολίες της Στ’ δημοτικού με αυτές των παιδιών χωρίς μαθησιακές δυσκολίες της Δ΄ τάξης. Παρόλα αυτά. τα αποτελέσματα έδειξαν επίσης ότι, παρά τις διαφορές τους, παιδιά με και χωρίς μαθησιακές δυσκολίες χρησιμοποιούν κοινές ορθογραφικές στρατηγικές ενώ αυτό που φαίνεται να προσδιορίζει το προφίλ των παιδιών με μαθησιακές δυσκολίες είναι η διατήρηση, παρά την ανάπτυξη της ορθογραφικής τους ικανότητας με την ηλικία, λαθών που αλλοιώνουν τη φωνολογική ταυτότητα των λέξεων που αποδίδουν ορθογραφικά. / The aim of this study was to compare the spelling performance of students of class 4th, class 5th and 6th class of primary school with learning difficulties with students of the same classes without learning difficulties, to yield orthographic lexical morphemes of Greek words. The survey took place 927 children. Of these 776 were children without learning disabilities and 151 were children with learning difficulties. The survey results, fully consistent with findings of other surveys, showed that the performance of children with learning difficulties was a statistically significant difference, lower than the children without difficulties of the same age and also showed that the difference remains even when comparing performance of children with learning difficulties of 6th class with those of children without learning difficulties of 4th class. Nevertheless, results also showed that despite their differences, children with and without learning disabilities using common spelling strategies and what seems to determine the profile of children with learning difficulties is to maintain, despite the development of spelling ability with age, errors that alter the identity of phonological give spelling words.
148

The use of phonological and orthographic information for memory and spelling : an analysis of reading and spelling subtypes

Harrison, Gina Louise 11 1900 (has links)
The present study was designed to examine differences between subtypes of readers and spellers in their performance on several phonological, orthographic, and memory tasks. A central question involved whether subtypes of readers and spellers could be distinguished based on their performance across the tasks administered. Based on their performance on a standardized achievement test, fourth and fifth grade children (N=50) were classified as having no difficulties with reading and spelling (good readers and spellers), difficulties with spelling, but not reading (mixed readers and spellers), or difficulties with both reading and spelling (poor readers and spellers). Each student was given a series of tasks to assess their use of phonological and orthographic information for memory and spelling. These tasks included: 1) rhyme judgment, 2) cued recall, 3) reading pronounceable pseudowords, 4) deciding which of. two pseudowords looks most like a real word, and 5) reporting on the kinds of strategies used to spell words. An error analysis was also conducted. Students with reading and spelling difficulties performed consistently lower than good and mixed readers and spellers on tasks assessing their use of phonological information. Good and mixed readers and spellers were not distinguishable on these tasks. Students with no reading and spelling difficulties or with spelling difficulties only performed better than poor readers and spellers on some tasks assessing orthographic processing. Specifically, mixed readers and spellers were distinguishable from good readers and spellers by their poorer recall of visually similar words. Good and poor subtypes were not distinguishable on this task. Poor readers and spellers also achieved comparable scores to the good and mixed readers and spellers on a measure of orthographic awareness. Overall results provided evidence supporting subtypes of reading and spelling ability groups. Students with no reading and spelling difficulties, or difficulties with spelling but not reading were similar in their use of phonological information. However, students with reading and spelling difficulties were more similar to the good readers and spellers in their use of orthographic information in memory. The findings from the present study have implications to subsequent research examining spelling ability, provide further evidence of the unique processing characteristics of the paradoxical good reader but poor speller, and suggest the possibility of unique programming needs to remediate spelling difficulties in mixed and poor readers and spellers.
149

A fifth year follow-up study of students who learned to read using I.F.A. in comparison to those using T.O.

DeGroff, Ruth Lavinia January 1972 (has links)
The purpose of the present study was to determine the achievement levels and expressed attitudes and feelings, five years later in 1972, of a group of students who learned to read in grade one in 1966-1967, using i.t.a. in comparison to a group who learned to read using T.O.Four areas of achievement, namely Reading Vocabulary, Reading Comprehension, Spelling, and Mathematics were tested by use of selected sub-sections of the California Achievement Test. A questionnaire was developed to determine the attitudes and feelings of students toward first grade reading experiences and their present attitudes and feelings toward reading in 1972.A Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test administered during the first grade was used as the entry level for the analysis of co-variance. Analysis of variance and covariance were the statistical techniques used to determine the differences between the groups that might be attributed to sex, method, or sex by method interaction.The student sample for the study consisted of 143 pupils from the Marion Community Schools, located in Marion, Indiana. The method of instruction of 67 of the pupils hadbeen i.t.a. in first grade while 76 pupils in the same schools had instruction with T.U. materials. The students have been exposed to various materials in the five years since their initial learning experiences, with random grouping of those who were initially presented i.t.a. or T.O.Data were analyzed to test the null hypotheses:There are no differences in levels of achievement of pupils who learned to read using i.t.a. and those using T.O. on either of the four dependent measures attributable to the following,1. sex2. method3. sex by method. interactionThe computed F ratios were not significant between sexes, methods, or sex by method interaction for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, spelling, and mathematics, therefore the null hypotheses could not be rejected, for these areas of achievement.There are no differences in levels of achievement of pupils who learned to read using i.t.a. and those using T.O. on either of the four dependent measures with the entry level as a co-variant attributable to the following,1. sex2. method3. sex by method interactionThe computed F ratios were not significant between methods or sex by method interaction for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, spelling, and mathematics when the entry level was used as a co-variant. The computed F ratios were not significant between sexes for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, and mathematics when the entry level was used as a co-variant. The computed F ratio was significant between sex for spelling when the entry level was used as a co-variant. The co-variant, entry level, was significant. The null hypotheses could not be rejected for differences between methods or sex by method interaction for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, spelling, and mathematics when the entry level was used as a co-variant. The null hypotheses could not be rejected for differences between sexes for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, and mathematics with the entry level as a co-variant. The null hypotheses for differences between sexes must be rejected for spelling with the entry level as a co-variant because significant differences were found.Data were analyzed to test the null hypotheses:There is no difference in attitude toward beginning reading learning as revealed by a questionnaire between pupils in the group who learned to read using i.t.a. and those using T.O. for,1. the total group2. the boys3. the girlsOnly slight differences were found in the responses to the part of the questionnaire concerning reading attitudes and feelings about learning to read. Therefore the null hypotheses could not be rejected.Data were analyzed to test the null hypotheses: There is no difference in attitude toward reading as revealed by a questionnaire between pupils in the group who learned to read using i.t.a. and those using T.O. for, 1. the total group2. the boys3. the girlsNo consistent differences in attitudes and feelings were revealed in answers to the part of the questionnaire concerning attitudes and feelings about reading at the present time (1972). Therefore the null hypotheses could not be rejected.For the pupils in this study it seems tenable to conclude that little difference exists in the achievement levels of reading, spelling, and mathematics between the group of students who learned to read using i.t.a. and the group who learned to read using T.O. after five years of study beyond the initial reading learning experiences.Likewise, little difference exists in the expressed attitudes and feelings of these groups.Spelling achievement levels were significantly different between sexes with the entry level as a co-variant. Girls then scored higher than boys regardless of the method.The entry level, a first grade intelligence test score, was significant for each area of achievement. Therefore it seems tenable to conclude that intelligence, as measured by this instrument, played an important part in the achievement of both groups. Students scoring higher on the intellectual measure also scored higher on achievement tests regardless of the method and students scoring poorer on the intellectual measure also scored poorer on the achievement test regardless of the method.
150

A quasi-experimental comparison of the test-study and study-test methods in fourth grade spelling

Bristor, Valerie Jayne January 1988 (has links)
The purpose of this study was to compare two methods of spelling instruction, the test-study method and study-test method, with the spelling achievement of selected fourth grade students. The effects of gender and spelling ability level on the spelling method were also studied. The participants were 80 fourth grade students from four intact classrooms in two elementary schools in a small suburban midwestern school district.Third grade standardized test scores were collected from students' permanent record cards and used for grouping students into spelling ability levels. A Spelling Criterion-Referenced Test was used as a pretest (covariate) and a posttest (dependent variable). An analysis of covariance was used to test three null hypotheses at the .05 level of significance. The three null hypotheses were not rejected. The following results were suggested:1. Both fourth grade boys and girls achieve equally well in spelling whether they use the test-study method or the study-test method of spelling instruction.2. Fourth grade students achieve equally well in spelling whether they use the test-study method or the study-test method of spelling instruction.3. Fourth grade girls and boys achieve equally well in spelling.Teachers should consider integrating spelling into all areas of the language arts by supplementing the spelling textbook with words the children are using in their writing. / Department of Elementary Education

Page generated in 0.0747 seconds