261 |
A disfluência comum e gaga / The common and stutterer dysfluencyVischi, Mariane Carvalho [UNESP] 03 March 2017 (has links)
Submitted by Mariane Carvalho Vischi (mazicarvalho@bol.com.br) on 2017-03-31T02:44:30Z
No. of bitstreams: 1
DEFESA - VERSÃO FINAL MARIANE CARVALHO VISCHI.pdf: 6742229 bytes, checksum: 05af4a4bb57ab550fefe7fac1686e50e (MD5) / Approved for entry into archive by Juliano Benedito Ferreira (julianoferreira@reitoria.unesp.br) on 2017-04-06T17:14:36Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1
vischi_mc_dr_arafcl.pdf: 6742229 bytes, checksum: 05af4a4bb57ab550fefe7fac1686e50e (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2017-04-06T17:14:36Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1
vischi_mc_dr_arafcl.pdf: 6742229 bytes, checksum: 05af4a4bb57ab550fefe7fac1686e50e (MD5)
Previous issue date: 2017-03-03 / Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) / O presente trabalho investiga e compara a disfluência na fala de pessoas gagas e não gagas. O primeiro tipo foi denominado de disfluência gaga e o segundo, de disfluência comum. De uma maneira geral, as disfluências são caracterizadas pelo não fluir normal dos segmentos sonoros da fala, devido à presença de hesitações e de interrupções durante o processo comunicativo. Inicialmente, foi feita uma revisão bibliográfica sobre a relação da fluência com a disfluência a partir de uma compilação dos principais estudos sobre o assunto. Na sequência, apresentamos os trabalhos da literatura especializada e da linguística para a disfluência gaga, bem como dos aspectos que tratam da articulação da fala e dos elementos suprassegmentais. A metodologia consistiu na gravação semiespontânea de seis informantes gagos e seis não gagos. Como parte da metodologia para a formação do corpus, foi pedido a eles que narrassem algum fato que tivesse marcado suas vidas. Não foi definida de uma temática específica. Os dados foram analisados auditivamente e acusticamente. Para a análise acústica utilizou-se o software PRAAT. Para análise e interpretação dos dados foram usados modelos teóricos referentes à fonologia autossegmental (Pierrehumbert,1980; Ladd, 1996, entre outros) e, como apoio teórico, o da fonologia funcional (Halliday, 1970; Cagliari, 2007). Também foram utilizados os trabalhos relacionados ao estudo da fluência e disfluência da fala de uma maneira geral (Scarpa, 1995; Cruz, 2009; Koch, 2009; Scarpa; Fernandes-Svartman, 2012; Merlo, 2015, entre outros), bem como da disfluência gaga (Van Riper, 1971; Friedman 1986; Azevedo, 2000, entre outros). A investigação acústica estuda os segmentos fonéticos, as pausas, as repetições, as hesitações, os alongamentos e os bloqueios de fala. A interpretação fonológica investiga o comportamento dos fonemas nos dois tipos de disfluências. Os resultados mostraram, em relação à análise da F0 e padrão da intensitade, diferenças pouco significativas para a comparação entre os dois tipos de disfluências. A duração foi o evento que mais distanciou os dois tipos de fala, sendo maior para os falantes com disfluência gaga. Em relação aos tipos de disfluências (repetições, inserções, hesitações, entre outros), tanto os informantes gagos quanto os não gagos realizaram o mesmo tipo de disfluência. A diferença entre eles deve-se à quantidade e à complexidade das disfluências. Nesse caso, os informantes gagos tendem a apresentar um maior número de disfluências por grupo tonal, bem como uma quantidade maior de disfluências complexas. Outra diferença entre os dois grupos está na duração e quantidade de pausas, que tende a ser maior para os informantes com disfluência gaga. A análise fonológica, por sua vez, mostrou que gagos e não gagos tendem a manter o padrão entoacional para as repetições, inserções e hesitações. Esses resultados confirmam a hipótese de que a gagueira estaria mais relacionada a uma questão articulatória que leva, por meio de uma empatia fonética negativa, a quebra na expectativa do interlocutor. Já a disfluência comum, por estar mais relacionada a fenômenos estilísticos, não é sentida da mesma maneira. Mesmo assim, o caminho mais adequado não é o de distanciar negativamente esses dois tipos de fala, uma vez que os eventos são os mesmos, o que muda é o modo como eles são refletidos e sentidos. / The present work investigates and compares the dysfluency speech of stuttering and non-stuttering people. The first type was called stutterer dysfluency and the second common disfluency. In general, dysfluencies are characterized by no expected flow of the phonetic segments of speech due to the presence of hesitations and interruptions during the communication process. A general review of the literature on the relationship between fluency and dysfluency is presented with comments. A special review presents works of linguists, discussing stutterer dysfluency in particular in relation to segmental and suprasegmental articulatory problems. The methodology consists of recordings of semi spontaneous speech by six stutterers and non-stutterers informants. As part of the methodology for the formation of the corpus, the informants were asked to report any fact that marked their lives. Data were analyzed auditorily and acoustically. For acoustic analysis, the PRAAT software was used. Theoretical models regarding autosegmental phonology (Pierrehumbert, 1980; Ladd, 1996 among others) and functional phonology (Halliday, 1970, Cagliari, 2007) were used for theoretical support. The study of fluency and dysfluency discusses in particular the works from Scarpa (1995); Cruz (2009); Koch (2009) and Merlo (2015). On the other hand, stutterer dysfluency reminds the works of Van Riper (1971); Friedman (1986); Azevedo (2000), among others. The acoustic analyses take into consideration the phonetic segments, pauses, repetitions, hesitations, speech stretching and blocking. The phonological interpretation investigates the phoneme behavior in both types of disfluency. The results revealed, in relation to the analysis of F0 and intensity standard, minor differences in comparing the two types of disfluency. The duration was the event that more distanced the two types of speech behavior, being higher for speakers with stutterer dysfluency. The difference between them is due to the amount of occurrences and the types of complex disfluencies. Stuttering informants tend to have a larger number of dysfluency by tonal group as well as a greater amount of complex dysfluency. The phonological analyses, in turn, showed that stuttering and non-stuttering tend to maintain the intonation pattern for repeats, insertions and hesitations. These results confirm the hypothesis that stuttering is more related to an articulation process which leads, through a negative phonetic empathy, to break the expectation of the interlocutor. On the other hand, common dysfluency which is more related to stylistic phenomena is also involved with the same kind of speech problems but in a different dimension. Indeed, the most appropriate conclusion is to set a scale for the events with the two types of disfluency in opposite positions. This scale is interpreted as such by the persons involved in the problem, being framed by different expectations. / CNPq: 141913/2013-4
|
262 |
Výzkum úrovně tvořivosti u dětí v 5. ročníku ZŠ / Research of the Creativity Level of Children of the Fifth GradeLETOVSKÁ, Petra January 2008 (has links)
The aim of the diploma thesis is to diagnostic levels of particular creative abilities in a selected group of population {--} pupils in the fifth grade. Theoretical knowledge is analyzed and compared in the first part. Particular creativity problems are briefly specified using specialized literature, e.g. Creativity barriers, Factors effecting creativity, Methods used for creativity development. Empiric part presents research description of individual levels in creative abilities. Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking were used as a main method of research. Effect of sex, class, school and its size on individual creativity parts was considered within the testing. A relative level of these parts was compared as well.
|
263 |
Neuromuscular Control Contributes to Incidental Learning: Head Orientation During Visual Statistical LearningJanuary 2013 (has links)
abstract: Incidental learning of sequential information occurs in visual, auditory and tactile domains. It occurs throughout our lifetime and even in nonhuman species. It is likely to be one of the most important foundations for the development of normal learning. To date, there is no agreement as to how incidental learning occurs. The goal of the present set of experiments is to determine if visual sequential information is learned in terms of abstract rules or stimulus-specific details. Two experiments test the extent to which interaction with the stimuli can influence the information that is encoded by the learner. The results of both experiments support the claim that stimulus and domain specific details directly shape what is learned, through a process of tuning the neuromuscular systems involved in the interaction between the learner and the materials. / Dissertation/Thesis / M.A. Psychology 2013
|
264 |
Influência dos parâmetros de soldagem na resistência à fluência de juntas soldadas de aço ASTM A 335 P91. / Influence of welding parameters on creep resistance of ASTM A 335 P91 steel welded joint.Diego Martins Garcia 03 May 2013 (has links)
A utilização do aço ASTM A 335 P91 tem sofrido um aumento considerável, especialmente em função da necessidade de materiais com maior resistência à fluência para aplicações em unidades de utilidades (geração de vapor) e de processo. Por ser um material de difícil soldabilidade, diversos problemas são enfrentados durante a construção das unidades e, posteriormente, durante manutenção, geralmente em função de problemas de soldagem oriundos da fase de fabricação e construção. As normas de projeto e construção utilizadas internacionalmente vêm sofrendo adaptações a fim de melhorar a utilização deste material, mas seus requisitos ainda apresentam importantes lacunas em relação às principais práticas recomendadas pela literatura técnica mais atual. Visando uma melhor utilização deste material, obtendo juntas soldadas com propriedades mecânicas e metalúrgicas mais consistentes, o presente trabalho foi desenvolvido avaliando-se a influência de parâmetros de soldagem energia de soldagem e temperatura de tratamento térmico pós-soldagem (TTPS) sobre as propriedades mecânicas e metalúrgicas, incluindo resistência à fluência, de juntas soldadas de aço ASTM A 335 P91. Foram testadas nove juntas soldadas, combinando três níveis de energia de soldagem com três temperaturas de TTPS. Os resultados obtidos indicam que é possível ter melhor controle sobre as propriedades deste material, desde que se opere com níveis mais restritos de energia de soldagem e maiores temperatura de TTPS, observando-se as limitações pertinentes, especialmente no que tange à temperatura crítica de transformação Ac1. / The use of ASTM A 335 P91 has been increased considerably specially because of the need for materials with higher creep resistance for power generation (nuclear and thermoelectric) and oil and gas processing applications. As this is a material with poor weldability, several issues are faced during construction of these unities, and later during maintenance usually associated with welding problems originated in the fabrication and construction phase. The design and construction codes most used worldwide are receiving important revisions targeting to enhance the use condition of this material but its requirements still show important lacks when compared to the main practices recommended by the more recent technical literature. Aiming a better usage of this material obtaining sound welded joints with more consistent metallurgical and mechanical properties, this work was developed assessing the influence of some welding parameters welding heat input and temperature of post-weld heat treatment (PWHT) on the metallurgical and mechanical properties, including creep strength, of welded joints of ASTM A 335 P91 steel. Nine welded joints were submitted to testing, combining three different levels of welding heat input with three different PWHT temperatures. The results show that it is possible to have a better control over the properties of this material since it is used lower welding heat inputs combined to higher PWHT temperatures, taking care about the limitations regarding the critical transformation temperature Ac1.
|
265 |
The Effects of Rate of Responding on Retention, Endurance, Stability, and Application of Performance on a Match-to-sample Task.Wheetley, Brook 12 1900 (has links)
Fluent performance has been described as the retention, endurance, stability, and application of the material learned. Fluent performers not only respond quickly during training, they also make many correct responses during training. The current study used a within-subject design to analyze the effects of increased response rates on Retention, Endurance, Stability, and Application tests. Number of correct responses and number of unprompted, correct responses in error correction procedures were yoked for individual participants across an Accuracy-plus-Rate training condition and an Accuracy-Only training condition. One participant scored better in tests that followed the Accuracy-Only condition. One participant showed results that slightly favor the Accuracy-plus-Rate training condition. The two participants whose response rates were successfully reduced in the Accuracy-Only condition performed better on all tests that followed the Accuracy-plus-Rate condition.
|
266 |
Retention, Endurance, Stability, and Application of Learned Performances as a Function of Training ConditionCohen, Jason 12 1900 (has links)
A functional definition of fluency describes performance frequency ranges that predict retention, endurance, stability, application, and adduction as outcomes of practice. This experiment assessed these outcomes after different training conditions using a within-subject design. Participants in an experimental group learned new skills in a condition with rate and accuracy criteria, then in a yoked, rate-controlled condition with the same number of prompted responses and correct trials in practice. Control group participants received training in consecutive conditions with rate and accuracy criteria. Performance of individuals in the control group demonstrated practice effects. Data obtained from participants in the experimental group showed similar performance across conditions. Considering efficiency, the condition with rate and accuracy criteria was superior.
|
267 |
Organized Semantic Fluency and Executive Functioning in an Adult Clinical Sample and a Community SampleChlipala, M. Linda 08 1900 (has links)
The study investigated an organized semantic fluency task, (the Controlled Animal Fluency Task - CAFT) as a measure of executive functioning (EF) in adults, and the relationship with instrumental activities of daily living (IADL). Participants (N = 266) consisted of a clinical sample (n = 142) utilizing neuropsychological assessment data collected at an outpatient psychological center, and a community sample (n = 124). The clinical sample was a heterogeneous mixed neurological group including a variety of health conditions and comorbid anxiety and depression. The CAFT Animals by Size demonstrated a significant positive correlation with Category Fluency (r = .71, n = 142, p < .001) , Animal Fluency (r = .70, n = 142, p < .001), and with other, established neuropsychological measures. The CAFT Animals by Size condition demonstrated a significant moderate negative correlation with IADL for the sample as a whole (r = -.46, n = 248, p < .001), and for the clinical sample (r = -.38, n = 129, p < .001), but not for the community sample. In a hierarchical regression analysis, CAFT Animal by Size explained additional variance in IADL (ΔR2 = .15). In a hierarchical regression analysis predicting IADL with the control variables entered first, followed by Category Fluency, with CAFT Animal by Size entered last, CAFT Animals by Size did not make a significant additional contribution. A stepwise forward regression indicated Category Fluency, education, and Category Switching are better predictors of IADL than CAFT Animals by Size. Normative data for the CAFT were calculated separately for age groups and education levels. Simple logistic regression indicated CAFT Animal by Size was a significant predictor of clinical or community group membership. A second logistic regression analysis indicated the CAFT Animal by Size condition improved the prediction of membership in the clinical versus the community group, compared to the MMSE alone. Applications of the CAFT are discussed.
|
268 |
Pre-Post Change in L2 Oral Fluency: the Lexico-Syntax of Large Fluency GainersDavid C Crouch (8767758) 27 April 2020 (has links)
<p>The theory underlying L2 oral fluency has focused on
cognitive processes, particularly proceduralization (Anderson, 1983; Levelt,
1989, 1999) and linguistic constructs, especially vocabulary and grammar
(Segalowitz, 2010). Towell, Hawkins, and Bazergui (1996) argued that
development of formulaic language enables automatic speech production. However,
no research has studied the longitudinal development of L2 oral fluency
concurrently with any of the following lexical variables: lexical frequency
profile, formulaic language use, and MTLD (a measure of lexical diversity). The
purpose of the present study is to clarify the process by which L2 oral
fluency, syntax, and vocabulary develop concurrently.</p>
<p>Data analysis involved three sequential phases: oral fluency
analysis, lexico-syntactic analysis, and discourse analysis. Oral fluency
measures were calculated using the transcribed oral test responses of 100
L1-Chinese EAP learners at the beginning and end of a required two-course EAP
language and culture sequence at Purdue University. The task completed was a
computer-administered, two-minute argumentative speaking task. This study
included eight oral fluency measures: speech rate, mean length of speech run,
articulation rate, phonation time ratio, mean length of silent pause, mean
length of filled pause, silent pause frequency, and filled pause frequency. For
the ten participants who made the largest percentage-wise oral fluency gains
(in terms of the oral fluency variable associated with the largest effect size
of gains), oral transcripts were analyzed to compute descriptive statistics for
the three lexical variables mentioned above and three syntactic variables: coordinate clause ratio, dependent clause
ratio, and words per T-unit. </p>
Results indicated significant change in all oral fluency measures,
except mean length of silent pause and mean length of filled pause. The largest
gains were made in mean length of speech run. Of the linguistic variables, the
largest longitudinal change was associated with coordinate clause ratio.
Discourse analysis of the transcripts of large fluency gainers' pre-post
responses suggested that large fluency gainers used coordinate clauses to build
more sophisticated discourse models in the post-test response than they did in
the pre-test response. Implications for L2 oral fluency theory, EAP pedagogy,
and L2 oral assessment are discussed.
|
269 |
Linguistic Profiles of High Proficiency Mandarin and Hindi Second Language Speakers of English.pdfJie Gao (8764734) 28 April 2020 (has links)
<div>This dissertation investigates three utterance fluency features and two vocabulary features of 409 speech samples from advanced intermediate and advanced L2 English speakers, who participated in the Oral English Proficiency Test (OEPT) between the year of 2009 and 2015. Among the 409 L2 English speakers, there are 80 L1 Hindi speakers rated as advanced intermediate, 32 L1 Hindi speakers rated as advanced, 286 L1 Mandarin speakers rated as advanced intermediate, and 11 L1 Mandarin speakers rated as advanced.</div><div><br></div><div>Hierarchical Cluster Analysis (HCA) was conducted and presented four different clusters among all the L2 English speakers. The four different clusters are: (1) Low Mean Syllables per Run (MSR), low Speech Rate (SR), very high Pause Rate (PR), medium Measure of Textual Lexical Diversity (MTLD), and medium percentage of words on the Academic Word List (AWL); (2) Medium Mean Syllables per Run (MSR), medium Speech Rate (SR), high Pause Rate (PR), low Measure of Textual Lexical Diversity (MTLD), and low percentage of words on the Academic Word List (AWL); (3) High Mean Syllables per Run (MSR), high Speech Rate (SR), low Pause Rate (PR), medium Measure of Textual Lexical Diversity (MTLD), and medium percentage of words on the Academic Word List (AWL); (4) Medium Mean Syllables per Run (MSR), medium Speech Rate (SR), low Pause Rate (PR), very high Measure of Textual Lexical Diversity, and very high percentage level of words on the Academic Word List (AWL).</div><div>Chi-square results show that L2 English speakers’ cluster membership is strongly associated with both their L1 background and level of L2 oral English proficiency. While most of the advanced intermediate L1 Mandarin speakers are in Cluster 1 and Cluster 2, the majority of the advanced intermediate L1 Hindi speakers concentrate in Cluster 3. A large number of advanced L1 Mandarin speakers and L1 Hindi speakers are also located in Cluster 3.</div><div><br></div><div>Twelve raters were invited to evaluate speech samples representative of the four clusters in terms of accent difference and listener effort. Twelve speakers were selected from the four clusters, whose speech samples have values of the five linguistic features closest to the cluster mean.</div><div><br></div><div>Multi-facet Rasch Measurement (MFRM) results show that L1 Mandarin speakers generally received lower ratings in accent difference and listener effort. The connection among fluency, vocabulary, and accentedness/listener effort, however, functions differently for L1 Mandarin speakers and L1 Hindi speakers. For advanced intermediate L1 Mandarin speakers, those who speak slower and use more diverse vocabulary and more academic words were evaluated to be less accented, meanwhile costing less listener effort. However, advanced intermediate L1 Hindi speakers were rated as less accented and cost less listener effort when they demonstrate higher fluency measures and lower vocabulary measures.</div><div><br></div><div>Advanced L2 English speakers, in contrary, received reverse rating results. The advanced L1 Mandarin speaker, who speaks faster and uses less diverse vocabulary and fewer academic words, was evaluated to be less accented and cost less listener effort. However, the advanced L1 Hindi speaker, who speaks slower and uses more diverse vocabulary and more academic words, was rated as less accented and cost less listener effort.</div><div><br></div><div>This dissertation reemphasizes that holistic rating rubric does not deny the existence of multiple linguistic profiles. Raters are sensitive to different combinations of fluency and vocabulary features even if they have been asked to use a holistic scale. In addition, L2 English speakers may adopt individual strategies to accommodate while delivering, which calls for further pedagogical attention.<br></div><div><br></div>
|
270 |
Enhancing Reading Fluency and Comprehension Using Stick Puppets in the LibrarySteele, L., Podyin, G., Dwyer, Edward J. 01 January 2010 (has links)
The authors suggest that students need to become physically as well as academically involved in their learning. In this light, there are procedures presented for having students make stick puppets that can be used in a variety of learning activities including, but not limited to, readers' theatre and story retelling. We propose that fables can provide a good foundation for developing activities featuring stick puppets, speaking and reading fluency oriented activities. We enncourage teachers and librarians to help make the puppets in order to incorporate first-hand knowledge of the procedures for their students.
|
Page generated in 0.0839 seconds