• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 13
  • 4
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 37
  • 37
  • 9
  • 8
  • 7
  • 7
  • 6
  • 5
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • 4
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
11

Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence for Hippocampal Involvement in Object Motion Processing in C57BL/6J Mice

Unknown Date (has links)
Considerable research has been carried out to establish a rodent model for the study of human memory, yet functional similarities between the species remain up for debate. The hippocampus, a region deep within the medial temporal lobe of the mammalian CNS, is critical for long-term episodic memory. Projections from the medial entorhinal cortex convey spatial/contextual information, while projections from the lateral entorhinal cortex convey item/object information to the hippocampus. The functional significance of these parallel projections to the rodent hippocampus has been suggested to support spatial processing, while the same projections to the human hippocampus support spatial and non-spatial memory. Discharging in a location-specific manner, hippocampal place cells contribute to spatial memory; however, evidence for neuronal correlates of non-spatial object memory has not been fully defined. The current experiments were designed to address the following questions, while utilizing electrophysiology, functional inactivation during a novel behavioral task, and immunohistochemistry. Is the memory for objects hippocampal-dependent, solely due to the location of the object, or are objects represented within hippocampal activity independent of location? To tease apart spatial and non-spatial processing by the hippocampus, the spatial aspects of 3D objects were enhanced by utilizing movement. A novel discriminatory avoidance task, Knowing Your Enemy, was adapted from an Enemy Avoidance task to test true object memory in mice. Current findings support the notion that object-associations acquisition depends upon a specific context. Retrieval of such object-associations is not context-dependent, yet remains sensitive to temporary inactivation of the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. The avoidance impairments observed following hippocampal inactivation were shown to not be a result of reduced anxiety. Immunohistochemical marker expression suggests that the CA1 region was highly active during object exposures, yet the hippocampal system responded differentially to moving and to stationary objects. Recordings of CA1 neurons yielded non-bursting object-related activity during object exploration, and place cell activity remained unaffected in the presence of moving objects; supporting independent, yet simultaneous processing of spatial and non-spatial information within the hippocampus. Together, the current findings support the notion that the CA1 region of the rodent hippocampus processes object-related information, independent of spatial information. / Includes bibliography. / Dissertation (Ph.D.)--Florida Atlantic University, 2017. / FAU Electronic Theses and Dissertations Collection
12

Partner response to verbal play in communication with individuals with amnesia

Miller, Margaret 01 May 2015 (has links)
Previous research into the communication of people with amnesia found that they, and their familiar communication partners, used verbal play less frequently than pairs without amnesia (Duff et al., 2009). This study attempts to analyze an additional dimension of playful language use: partner response to verbal play. A rubric was developed to rate verbal play response on a 0-5 scale. The rubric was used to rate partner response in four communication pairs containing one partner with amnesia and one familiar communication partner; and four healthy pairs for comparison. The responses of the experimenter participating in the conversations were also rated. While the study found no differences between the two groups in terms of the familiar communication partners or the experimenter, the participants with amnesia received significantly lower overall scores than participants without amnesia. The participants with amnesia also produced a significantly lower proportion of responses with multiple turns than did healthy participants. This result adds to the body of evidence that memory disorders can affect social interaction. The rubric developed for this study suggests a possible direction for including partner response in analyses of conversational discourse.
13

Semantische Repräsentation, obligatorische Aktivierung und verbale Produktion arithmetischer Fakten / Semantic representation, obligatory activation, and verbal production of arithmetic facts

Domahs, Frank January 2006 (has links)
Die vorliegende Arbeit widmet sich der Repräsentation und Verarbeitung arithmetischer Fakten. Dieser Bereich semantischen Wissens eignet sich unter anderem deshalb besonders gut als Forschungsgegenstand, weil nicht nur seine einzelne Bestandteile, sondern auch die Beziehungen dieser Bestandteile untereinander außergewöhnlich gut definierbar sind. Kognitive Modelle können also mit einem Grad an Präzision entwickelt werden, der in anderen Bereichen kaum je zu erreichen sein wird. Die meisten aktuellen Modelle stimmen darin überein, die Repräsentation arithmetischer Fakten als eine assoziative, netzwerkartig organisierte Struktur im deklarativen Gedächtnis zu beschreiben. Trotz dieser grundsätzlichen Übereinstimmung bleibt eine Reihe von Fragen offen. In den hier vorgestellten Untersuchungen werden solche offene Fragen in Hinsicht auf drei verschiedene Themenbereiche angegangen: 1) die neuroanatomischen Korrelate 2) Nachbarschaftskonsistenzeffekte bei der verbalen Produktion sowie 3) die automatische Aktivierung arithmetischer Fakten. In einer kombinierten fMRT- und Verhaltensstudie wurde beispielsweise der Frage nachgegangen, welche neurofunktionalen Entsprechungen es für den Erwerb arithmetischer Fakten bei Erwachsenen gibt. Den Ausgangspunkt für diese Untersuchung bildete das Triple-Code-Modell von Dehaene und Cohen, da es als einziges auch Aussagen über neuroanatomische Korrelate numerischer Leistungen macht. Das Triple-Code-Modell geht davon aus, dass zum Abruf arithmetischer Fakten eine „perisylvische“ Region der linken Hemisphäre unter Einbeziehung der Stammganglien sowie des Gyrus angularis nötig ist (Dehaene & Cohen, 1995; Dehaene & Cohen, 1997; Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003). In der aktuellen Studie sollten gesunde Erwachsene komplexe Multiplikationsaufgaben etwa eine Woche lang intensiv üben, so dass ihre Beantwortung immer mehr automatisiert erfolgt. Die Lösung dieser geübten Aufgaben sollte somit – im Gegensatz zu vergleichbaren ungeübten Aufgaben – immer stärker auf Faktenabruf als auf der Anwendung von Prozeduren und Strategien beruhen. Hingegen sollten ungeübte Aufgaben im Vergleich zu geübten höhere Anforderungen an exekutive Funktionen einschließlich des Arbeitsgedächtnisses stellen. Nach dem Training konnten die Teilnehmer – wie erwartet – geübte Aufgaben deutlich schneller und sicherer beantworten als ungeübte. Zusätzlich wurden sie auch im Magnetresonanztomografen untersucht. Dabei konnte zunächst bestätigt werden, dass das Lösen von Multiplikationsaufgaben allgemein von einem vorwiegend linkshemisphärischen Netzwerk frontaler und parietaler Areale unterstützt wird. Das wohl wichtigste Ergebnis ist jedoch eine Verschiebung der Hirnaktivierungen von eher frontalen Aktivierungsmustern zu einer eher parietalen Aktivierung und innerhalb des Parietallappens vom Sulcus intraparietalis zum Gyrus angularis bei den geübten im Vergleich zu den ungeübten Aufgaben. So wurde die zentrale Bedeutung von Arbeitsgedächtnis- und Planungsleistungen für komplexe ungeübte Rechenaufgaben erneut herausgestellt. Im Sinne des Triple-Code-Modells könnte die Verschiebung innerhalb des Parietallappens auf einen Wechsel von quantitätsbasierten Rechenleistungen (Sulcus intraparietalis) zu automatisiertem Faktenabruf (linker Gyrus angularis) hindeuten. Gibt es bei der verbalen Produktion arithmetischer Fakten Nachbarschaftskonsistenzeffekte ähnlich zu denen, wie sie auch in der Sprachverarbeitung beschrieben werden? Solche Effekte sind nach dem aktuellen „Dreiecksmodell“ von Verguts & Fias (2004) zur Repräsentation von Multiplikationsfakten erwartbar. Demzufolge sollten richtige Antworten leichter gegeben werden können, wenn sie Ziffern mit möglichst vielen semantisch nahen falschen Antworten gemeinsam haben. Möglicherweise sollten demnach aber auch falsche Antworten dann mit größerer Wahrscheinlichkeit produziert werden, wenn sie eine Ziffer mit der richtigen Antwort teilen. Nach dem Dreiecksmodell wäre darüber hinaus sogar der klassische Aufgabengrößeneffekt bei einfachen Multiplikationsaufgaben (Zbrodoff & Logan, 2004) auf die Konsistenzverhältnisse der richtigen Antwort mit semantisch benachbarten falschen Antworten zurückzuführen. In einer Reanalyse der Fehlerdaten von gesunden Probanden (Campbell, 1997) und einem Patienten (Domahs, Bartha, & Delazer, 2003) wurden tatsächlich Belege für das Vorhandensein von Zehnerkonsistenzeffekten beim Lösen einfacher Multiplikationsaufgaben gefunden. Die Versuchspersonen bzw. der Patient hatten solche falschen Antworten signifikant häufiger produziert, welche die gleiche Zehnerziffer wie das richtigen Ergebnisses aufwiesen, als ansonsten vergleichbare andere Fehler. Damit wird die Annahme unterstützt, dass die Zehner- und die Einerziffern zweistelliger Zahlen separate Repräsentationen aufweisen – bei der Multiplikation (Verguts & Fias, 2004) wie auch allgemein bei numerischer Verarbeitung (Nuerk, Weger, & Willmes, 2001; Nuerk & Willmes, 2005). Zusätzlich dazu wurde in einer Regressionsanalyse über die Fehlerzahlen auch erstmalig empirische Evidenz für die Hypothese vorgelegt, dass der klassische Aufgabengrößeneffekt beim Abruf von Multiplikationsfakten auf Zehnerkonsistenzeffekte zurückführbar ist: Obwohl die Aufgabengröße als erster Prädiktor in das Modell einging, wurde diese Variable wieder verworfen, sobald ein Maß für die Nachbarschaftskonsistenz der richtigen Antwort in das Modell aufgenommen wurde. Schließlich wurde in einer weiteren Studie die automatische Aktivierung von Multiplikationsfakten bei gesunden Probanden mit einer Zahlenidentifikationsaufgabe (Galfano, Rusconi, & Umilta, 2003; Lefevre, Bisanz, & Mrkonjic, 1988; Thibodeau, Lefevre, & Bisanz, 1996) untersucht. Dabei sollte erstmals die Frage beantwortet werden, wie sich die automatische Aktivierung der eigentlichen Multiplikationsergebnisse (Thibodeau et al., 1996) zur Aktivierung benachbarter falscher Antworten (Galfano et al., 2003) verhält. Ferner sollte durch die Präsentation mit verschiedenen SOAs der zeitliche Verlauf dieser Aktivierungen aufgeklärt werden. Die Ergebnisse dieser Studie können insgesamt als Evidenz für das Vorhandensein und die automatische, obligatorische Aktivierung eines Netzwerkes arithmetischer Fakten bei gesunden, gebildeten Erwachsenen gewertet werden, in dem die richtigen Produkte stärker mit den Faktoren assoziiert sind als benachbarte Produkte (Operandenfehler). Dabei führen Produkte kleiner Aufgaben zu einer stärkeren Interferenz als Produkte großer Aufgaben und Operandenfehler großer Aufgaben zu einer stärkeren Interferenz als Operandenfehler kleiner Aufgaben. Ein solches Aktivierungsmuster passt gut zu den Vorhersagen des Assoziationsverteilungsmodells von Siegler (Lemaire & Siegler, 1995; Siegler, 1988), bei dem kleine Aufgaben eine schmalgipflige Verteilung der Assoziationen um das richtige Ergebnis herum aufweisen, große Aufgaben jedoch eine breitgipflige Verteilung. Somit sollte die vorliegende Arbeit etwas mehr Licht in bislang weitgehend vernachlässigte Aspekte der Repräsentation und des Abrufs arithmetischer Fakten gebracht haben: Die neuronalen Korrelate ihres Erwerbs, die Konsequenzen ihrer Einbindung in das Stellenwertsystem mit der Basis 10 sowie die spezifischen Auswirkungen ihrer assoziativen semantischen Repräsentation auf ihre automatische Aktivierbarkeit. Literatur Campbell, J. I. (1997). On the relation between skilled performance of simple division and multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1140-1159. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83-120. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33, 219-250. Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 487-506. Domahs, F., Bartha, L., & Delazer, M. (2003). Rehabilitation of arithmetic abilities: Different intervention strategies for multiplication. Brain and Language, 87, 165-166. Galfano, G., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. (2003). Automatic activation of multiplication facts: evidence from the nodes adjacent to the product. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 56, 31-61. Lefevre, J. A., Bisanz, J., & Mrkonjic, L. (1988). Cognitive arithmetic: evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts. Memory and Cognition, 16, 45-53. Lemaire, P. & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83-97. Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25-B33. Nuerk, H. C. & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Science, 47, 52-72. Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 258-275. Thibodeau, M. H., Lefevre, J. A., & Bisanz, J. (1996). The extension of the interference effect to multiplication. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 393-396. Verguts, T. & Fias, W. (2004). Neighborhood Effects in Mental Arithmetic. Psychology Science. Zbrodoff, N. J. & Logan, G. D. (2004). What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Hrsg.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp.331-345). New York, NY: Psychology Press. / The present thesis deals with the representation and processing of arithmetic facts. This domain of semantic knowledge has gained a substantial amount of interest as its components as well as their interrelations are well specified. Thus, cognitive models can be developed with a degree of precision, which cannot be reached in many other domains. Most recent models agree that arithmetic facts are represented in an associative, network-like structure in declarative memory. Despite this general agreement a lot of issues still remain unresolved. The open questions tackled in the present work address three different aspects of arithmetic facts: 1) their neuro-anatomical correlates, 2) neighbourhood consistency effects in their verbal production and 3) their automatic activation. In a combined behavioural and fMRI study the neurofunctional correlates of the acquisition of arithmetic facts in adults were examined. This research was based on the Triple-Code-Model of Dehaene and Cohen, the only recent model which makes explicit assumptions on neuroanatomical correlates of numerical abilities. The Triple-Code-Model assumes that a “perisylvian” region in the left hemisphere including the basal ganglia and the Angular Gyrus is involved in the retrieval of arithmetic facts (Dehaene & Cohen, 1995; Dehaene & Cohen, 1997; Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003). In the present study healthy adults were asked to train complex multiplication problems extensively during one week. Thus, these problems could be solved more and more automatically. It was reasoned that answering these trained problems should more and more rely on the retrieval of facts from declarative memory, whereas answering untrained problems should rely on the application of strategies and procedures, which impose high demands on executive functions including working memory. After the training was finished, participants – as expected – could solve trained problems faster and more accurately than non-trained problems. Participants were also submitted to a functional magnetic resonance imaging examination. In general, this examination added to the evidence for a mainly left hemispheric fronto-parietal network being involved in mental multiplication. Crucially, comparing trained with non-trained problems a shift of activation from frontal to more parietal regions was observed. Thus, the central role of central executive and working memory for complex calculation was highlighted. Moreover, a shift of activation from the Intraparietal Sulcus to the Angular Gyrus took place within the parietal lobe. According to the Triple-Code-Model, this shift may be interpreted to indicate a strategy change from quantity based calculation, relying on the Intraparietal Sulcus, to fact retrieval, relying on the left Angular Gyrus. Are there neighbourhood consistency effects in the verbal production of arithmetic facts similar to what has been described for language production? According to the “Triangle Model” of simple multiplication, proposed by Verguts & Fias (2004), such effects can be expected. According to this model corrects answers can be given more easily if they share digits with many semantically close wrong answers. Moreover, it can be assumed that wrong answers, too, are more likely to be produced if they share a digit with the correct result. In addition to this, the Triangle Model also states that the classical problem size effect in simple multiplication (Zbrodoff & Logan, 2004) can be drawn back to neighbourhood consistency between the correct result and semantically close wrong answers. In fact, a re-analysis of error data from a sample of healthy young adults (Campbell, 1997) and a patient with acalculia (Domahs, Bartha, & Delazer, 2003) provided evidence for the existence of decade consistency effects in the verbal production of multiplication results. Healthy participants and the patient produced significantly more wrong answers which shared the decade digit with the correct result than otherwise comparable wrong answers. This result supports the assumption of separate representations of decade and unit digits in two-digit numbers in multiplication (Verguts & Fias, 2004) and in number processing in general (Nuerk, Weger, & Willmes, 2001; Nuerk & Willmes, 2005). Moreover, an additional regression analysis on the error rates provided first empirical evidence for the hypothesis that the classical problem size effect in the retrieval of multiplication facts may be an artefact of neighbourhood consistency: Although problem size was the first variable to enter the model, it was excluded from the model once a measure for neighbourhood consistency was included. Finally, in a further study the automatic activation of multiplication facts was examined in a number matching task (Galfano, Rusconi, & Umilta, 2003; Lefevre, Bisanz, & Mrkonjic, 1988; Thibodeau, Lefevre, & Bisanz, 1996). This experiment addressed the question how the automatic activation of actual multiplication results (Thibodeau et al., 1996) relates to the activation of semantically close wrong answers (Galfano et al., 2003). Furthermore, using different SOAs the temporal properties of these activations should be disclosed. In general, the results of this study provide evidence for an obligatory and automatic activation of a network of arithmetic facts in healthy educated adults in which correct results are stronger associated with the operands than semantically related wrong answers. Crucially, products of small problems lead to stronger interference effects than products of larger problems while operand errors of large problems lead to stronger interference effects than operand errors of small problems. Such a pattern of activation is in line with predictions of Siegler’s Distribution of Associations Model (Lemaire & Siegler, 1995; Siegler, 1988) which assumes a more peaked distribution of associations between operands and potential results for small compared to large multiplication problems. In sum, the present thesis should shed some light into largely ignored aspects of arithmetic fact retrieval: The neural correlates of its acquisition, the consequences of its implementation in the base 10 place value system, as well as the specific effects of its semantic representation for automatic activation of correct multiplication facts and related results. References Campbell, J. I. (1997). On the relation between skilled performance of simple division and multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1140-1159. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83-120. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33, 219-250. Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 487-506. Domahs, F., Bartha, L., & Delazer, M. (2003). Rehabilitation of arithmetic abilities: Different intervention strategies for multiplication. Brain and Language, 87, 165-166. Galfano, G., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. (2003). Automatic activation of multiplication facts: evidence from the nodes adjacent to the product. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 56, 31-61. Lefevre, J. A., Bisanz, J., & Mrkonjic, L. (1988). Cognitive arithmetic: evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts. Memory and Cognition, 16, 45-53. Lemaire, P. & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83-97. Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25-B33. Nuerk, H. C. & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Science, 47, 52-72. Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 258-275. Thibodeau, M. H., Lefevre, J. A., & Bisanz, J. (1996). The extension of the interference effect to multiplication. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 393-396. Verguts, T. & Fias, W. (2004). Neighborhood Effects in Mental Arithmetic. Psychology Science. Zbrodoff, N. J. & Logan, G. D. (2004). What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp.331-345). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
14

Predictive eyes precede retrieval : visual recognition as hypothesis testing

Holm, Linus January 2007 (has links)
Does visual recognition entail verifying an idea about what is perceived? This question was addressed in the three studies of this thesis. The main hypothesis underlying the investigation was that visual recognition is an active process involving hypothesis testing. Recognition of faces (Study 1), scenes (Study 2) and objects (Study 3) was investigated using eye movement registration as a window on the recognition process. In Study 1, a functional relationship between eye movements and face recognition was established. Restricting the eye movements reduced recognition performance. In addition, perceptual reinstatement as indicated by eye movement consistency across study and test was related to recollective experience at test. Specifically, explicit recollection was related to higher eye movement consistency than familiarity-based recognition and false rejections (Studies 1-2). Furthermore, valid expectations about a forthcoming stimulus scene produced eye movements which were more similar to those of an earlier study episode, compared to invalid expectations (Study 2). In Study 3 participants recognized fragmented objects embedded in nonsense fragments. Around 8 seconds prior to explicit recognition, participants began to fixate the object region rather than a similar control region in the stimulus pictures. Before participants’ indicated awareness of the object, they fixated it with an average of 9 consecutive fixations. Hence, participants were looking at the object as if they had recognized it before they became aware of its identity. Furthermore, prior object information affected eye movement sampling of the stimulus, suggesting that semantic memory was involved in guiding the eyes during object recognition even before the participants were aware of its presence. Collectively, the studies support the view that gaze control is instrumental to visual recognition performance and that visual recognition is an interactive process between memory representation and information sampling.
15

Procedural and Declarative Memory in Children with Developmental Disorders of Language and Literacy

Hedenius, Martina January 2013 (has links)
The procedural deficit hypothesis (PDH) posits that a range of language, cognitive and motor impairments associated with specific language impairment (SLI) and developmental dyslexia (DD) may be explained by an underlying domain-general dysfunction of the procedural memory system. In contrast, declarative memory is hypothesized to remain intact and to play a compensatory role in the two disorders. The studies in the present thesis were designed to test this hypothesis. Study I examined non-language procedural memory, specifically implicit sequence learning, in children with SLI. It was shown that children with poor performance on tests of grammar were impaired at consolidation of procedural memory compared to children with normal grammar. These findings support the PDH and are line with previous studies suggesting a link between grammar processing and procedural memory. In Study II, the same implicit sequence learning paradigm was used to test procedural memory in children with DD. The DD group showed a learning profile that was similar to that of children with SLI in Study I, with a significant impairment emerging late in learning, after extended practice and including an overnight interval. Further analyses suggested that the DD impairment may not be related to overnight consolidation but to the effects of further practice beyond the initial practice session. In contrast to the predictions of the PDH, the sequence learning deficit was unrelated to phonological processing skills as assessed with a nonword repetition task. Study III examined declarative memory in DD. The performance of the DD group was found to be not only intact, but even enhanced, compared to that of the control children. The results encourage further studies on the potential of declarative memory to compensate for the reading problems in DD. In sum, the results lend partial support for the PDH and suggest further refinements to the theory. Collectively, the studies emphasize the importance of going beyond a narrow focus on language learning and memory functions in the characterization of the two disorders. Such a broader cognitive, motor and language approach may inform the development of future clinical and pedagogical assessment and intervention practices for SLI and DD.
16

Participação da via NTS-PGI-LC-hipocampo (núcleo do trato solitário- núcleo paragigantocelular-Locus coeruleus-hipocampo) na consolidação da memória de reconhecimento de objetos

Carpes, Pâmela Billig Mello January 2010 (has links)
Existem crescentes evidências sobre a contribuição da liberação de noradrenalina (NA) central na consolidação das memórias. Teoricamente, o Núcleo do Trato Solitário (NTS) recebe informações e diversos estímulos periféricos, que são então projetados ao Núcleo Paragigantocelular (PGi). Este, por sua vez, utiliza neurotransmissores, predominantemente excitatórios, para influenciar a ativação do Locus Coeruleus (LC). Então, o LC envia projeções noradrenérgicas ao hipocampo e à amígdala, influenciando os processos mnemônicos. Aqui nós demonstramos que a inibição pelo muscimol do NTS, PGi ou LC até 3 horas após o treino na tarefa de reconhecimento de objetos (RO) impede a consolidação da memória medida 24 h após o treino. Adicionalmente, a infusão de timolol, um antagonista de receptores β-adrenérgicos, na região CA1 do hipocampo também impede a consolidação deste tipo de memória. A infusão de NA na região CA1 do hipocampo não altera a retenção da memória, mas, reverte o prejuízo causado pela inibição do NTS, PGi ou LC. A infusão de NMDA no LC após a inibição do NTS ou PGi também reverte essa amnésia. Concomitantemente, verificamos que a inibição NTS, PGi ou LC bloqueia o aumento da expressão do fator neurotrófico derivado do cérebro (BDNF, do inglês brain-derived neurotrophic factor) que ocorre 120 min após o treino na tarefa de reconhecimento de objetos na região CA1 do hipocampo. Também a infusão de NA na região CA1 do hipocampo após a inibição do NTS, PGi ou LC ou de NMDA no LC após a inibição do NTS ou PGi promovem novamente o aumento do BDNF120 min após o treino no RO. Com isso conclui-se que a ativação da via NTS-PGi-LC-Hipocampo é necessária para que ocorra consolidação da memória de RO, na qual desempenha um papel o BDNF hipocampal. / There is evidence of the contribution of brain noradrenaline release (NA) to memory consolidation. The Nucleus of the Solitary Tract (NTS) receives information originated by peripheral stimuli and projects to the Paragigantocellularis Nucleus (PGi), which influences the Locus Coeruleus (LC) through excitatory neurotransmitters. The LC sends noradrenergic projections to the hippocampus and amygdala, influencing the memory processes. Here we show that inhibition by muscimol of NTS, PGi or LC up to 3 h after object recognition training impairs the consolidation of the memory measured 24 h later. Additionally, the infusion of timolol in the CA1 region of hippocampus also inhibits consolidation of this type of memory. The infusion of NA into the CA1 region of hippocampus does not alter memory consolidation of this task, but reverts the deleterious effect of NTS, PGi or LC inhibition. The infusion of NMDA in LC after inhibition of NTS or PGi also reverts the amnesia. Concomitantly, the inhibition of NTS, PGi or LC blocks the increase of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in CA1 that occurs 120 min after training in the object recognition task. Further, the infusion of NA in CA1 after inhibition of NTS, PGi or LC; or of NMDA in LC after inhibition of NTS or PGi promotes the BDNF increase seen 120 min after object recognition training. Thus, it is concluded that the activation of NTSPGi- LC-Hippocampus pathway is necessary for consolidation of the object recognition memory, and hippocampal BDNF is involved in this process.
17

Participação da via NTS-PGI-LC-hipocampo (núcleo do trato solitário- núcleo paragigantocelular-Locus coeruleus-hipocampo) na consolidação da memória de reconhecimento de objetos

Carpes, Pâmela Billig Mello January 2010 (has links)
Existem crescentes evidências sobre a contribuição da liberação de noradrenalina (NA) central na consolidação das memórias. Teoricamente, o Núcleo do Trato Solitário (NTS) recebe informações e diversos estímulos periféricos, que são então projetados ao Núcleo Paragigantocelular (PGi). Este, por sua vez, utiliza neurotransmissores, predominantemente excitatórios, para influenciar a ativação do Locus Coeruleus (LC). Então, o LC envia projeções noradrenérgicas ao hipocampo e à amígdala, influenciando os processos mnemônicos. Aqui nós demonstramos que a inibição pelo muscimol do NTS, PGi ou LC até 3 horas após o treino na tarefa de reconhecimento de objetos (RO) impede a consolidação da memória medida 24 h após o treino. Adicionalmente, a infusão de timolol, um antagonista de receptores β-adrenérgicos, na região CA1 do hipocampo também impede a consolidação deste tipo de memória. A infusão de NA na região CA1 do hipocampo não altera a retenção da memória, mas, reverte o prejuízo causado pela inibição do NTS, PGi ou LC. A infusão de NMDA no LC após a inibição do NTS ou PGi também reverte essa amnésia. Concomitantemente, verificamos que a inibição NTS, PGi ou LC bloqueia o aumento da expressão do fator neurotrófico derivado do cérebro (BDNF, do inglês brain-derived neurotrophic factor) que ocorre 120 min após o treino na tarefa de reconhecimento de objetos na região CA1 do hipocampo. Também a infusão de NA na região CA1 do hipocampo após a inibição do NTS, PGi ou LC ou de NMDA no LC após a inibição do NTS ou PGi promovem novamente o aumento do BDNF120 min após o treino no RO. Com isso conclui-se que a ativação da via NTS-PGi-LC-Hipocampo é necessária para que ocorra consolidação da memória de RO, na qual desempenha um papel o BDNF hipocampal. / There is evidence of the contribution of brain noradrenaline release (NA) to memory consolidation. The Nucleus of the Solitary Tract (NTS) receives information originated by peripheral stimuli and projects to the Paragigantocellularis Nucleus (PGi), which influences the Locus Coeruleus (LC) through excitatory neurotransmitters. The LC sends noradrenergic projections to the hippocampus and amygdala, influencing the memory processes. Here we show that inhibition by muscimol of NTS, PGi or LC up to 3 h after object recognition training impairs the consolidation of the memory measured 24 h later. Additionally, the infusion of timolol in the CA1 region of hippocampus also inhibits consolidation of this type of memory. The infusion of NA into the CA1 region of hippocampus does not alter memory consolidation of this task, but reverts the deleterious effect of NTS, PGi or LC inhibition. The infusion of NMDA in LC after inhibition of NTS or PGi also reverts the amnesia. Concomitantly, the inhibition of NTS, PGi or LC blocks the increase of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in CA1 that occurs 120 min after training in the object recognition task. Further, the infusion of NA in CA1 after inhibition of NTS, PGi or LC; or of NMDA in LC after inhibition of NTS or PGi promotes the BDNF increase seen 120 min after object recognition training. Thus, it is concluded that the activation of NTSPGi- LC-Hippocampus pathway is necessary for consolidation of the object recognition memory, and hippocampal BDNF is involved in this process.
18

The effect of interference on reactivation of spatial memories in reconsolidation model by using an innovative experimental paradigm in healthy young adults at the behavioral level

Farrahi Moghaddam, Jeiran 04 1900 (has links)
No description available.
19

Memory Performance in Children with Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: Neocortical vs. Dual Pathologies

Korman, Brandon M. 01 January 2016 (has links)
This study investigated memory in children with temporal lobe epilepsy and the ability to discern hippocampal dysfunction with conventional memory tests that are typically used to detect more global memory impairment. All data was obtained retrospectively from the epilepsy surgery program at a local children’s hospital. The research population consisted of 54 children with intractable epilepsy of temporal onset, balanced across pathology types (with and without hippocampal disease) and other demographics. Each was given a clinical battery prior to surgical intervention, which included the WRAML/WRAML2 Verbal Learning subtest from which the dependent variables for this study were extracted. The research hypothesis had predicted that memory retention between verbal learning and recall would be worse for participants with pathology that included hippocampal sclerosis than for those with non-hippocampal temporal lobe pathology. A two-way mixed-design ANOVA was used to test the hypothesis, which allowed incorporation of variables of interest related to memory factors, pathology type, and hemispheric laterality, as well as their various interactions. There was a significant main effect for change in the number of words retained from the final learning trial to the delayed recall. Although the interaction between memory retention and pathology type was not statistically significant, the average of the memory scores as it related to pathology by side did show significance. Thus, results did not support the hypothetical relationship between retention and hippocampal function. However, additional exploratory analyses revealed that the final learning trial by itself was associated with hippocampal pathology, which applied only to those participants with left-hemisphere lesions. Logistic regression with the final learning trial correctly classified 74 percent of participants into the appropriate pathology category, with 81 percent sensitivity to hippocampal dysfunction. Mean participant memory scores were nearly one standard deviation below the normative mean for both delayed recall and total learning scaled scores, regardless of pathology type or lesion hemisphericity. Thus, while the conventionally used indices of the WRAML Verbal Learning test are useful for determining overall memory status, they are not specific to pathological substrate. The within-subject main effect showed an expected loss of information across the time of the delay, but overall the recall score showed no association with hippocampal functioning. This study revealed the possibility of measuring hippocampal function at statistically significant group levels using learning scores from a widely used measure of verbal memory, even in participants with intact contralateral mesial temporal structures. It also indicated that hippocampal structures do not play a role during recall measures given after a standard time delay. Data further demonstrated a role of the hippocampus for encoding and transferring information beyond short term/working memory into long term. During the learning process, the hippocampus appears to work in concert with short-term memory systems, but does not take over the encoding process until enough repetitions have occurred to saturate the working memory buffer. This research represents a small, yet important step forward in our understanding of the hippocampus, with potentially important implications for the future study of memory constructs and mensuration.
20

The relationship of self-reported reading habits and declarative memory

Álvarez, Bernardo January 2016 (has links)
The present study examined the possibility that reading books might support declarative memory, and potentially contribute to the cognitive reserve and thereby minimize age-related decline in memory functions. It was a crosssectional study, where data were taken from 566 Betula Study participants, as well as the scores of declarative memory assessment tasks, and book reading reports. The tasks of episodic memory were sentence learning with and without encoding enactment –free and cued recall. The tasks of semantic memory were word fluency, initial letter A, M fivewords, B profession names, and SRB –vocabulary. Three-way analysis of variance was performed for hypothesis testing. The results showed that book reading might support declarative memory. There was a significant relationship between education and book reading. Was not found if book reading might minimize age-related decline. In conclusion, the study showed that book reading and years of education might support declarative memory. / Denna studie undersökte om läsandet av böcker stödjer deklarativt minne, och eventuellt främjar den kognitivareserven, vilket medför en minimerad åldersbaserad reduktion i minnesfunktioner. I denna tvärsnittsstudie ingick datafrån 566 deltagare i Betulaprojektet som testats med avseende på deklarativa minnesfunktioner, samt enlitteraturanalys. Utvärderingarna som baserades på episodiskt minne var; menings-inlärning med och utan 'encodingenactment' -både fri och stödd återgivning. Test av semantiskt minne var ordflöde med första bokstav A, M fem-ord,B yrkesnamn, och SRB-ordförråd. Tre-vägs variansanalyer genomfördes för hypotestestning. Resultaten visade på enkoppling mellan utbildning och läsandet av böcker. Det är dock fortfarande oklart om läsandet av böcker minimeraråldersbaserad minnes-reducering. Sammanfattningsvis, visade studien att läsning och utbildning kan stödja deklarativtminne.

Page generated in 0.0934 seconds