Spelling suggestions: "subject:"generatedcomparative anda general"" "subject:"generatedcomparative ando general""
481 |
Markedness and the acquisition of pied-piping and preposition strandingFrench, Margot Anne. January 1985 (has links)
No description available.
|
482 |
Language indexation : a syntactic constraint on code-mixingMiller, Amanda January 1993 (has links)
No description available.
|
483 |
The syntax of sentential negation : interactions with case, agreement, and (in)definitenessDe Freitas, Leslie J. (Leslie Jill) January 1993 (has links)
No description available.
|
484 |
Rapprochement entre l'ablatif latin et l'instrumental russeBouchard, Marie January 1977 (has links)
No description available.
|
485 |
Transitivity alternations, event-types and light verbsAmberber, Mengistu January 1996 (has links)
No description available.
|
486 |
Syllable structure processes in Northern Sotho : a linear and non-linear phonological analysisMadigoe, Mashikane William 12 1900 (has links)
Thesis (MA)--Stellenbosch University, 2004. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: This study intends to describe and explain syllable structure processes in Northern Sotho.
It deals with phonological processes such as vowel deletion, semivocalization and
semivowel insertion. The major aim of these processes is to restore the preferred ICVI
syllable structure which has been violated by morphological processes such as passive,
diminutive, the construction of absolute pronouns, etc.
Two phonological models are applied with the intention to determine the one that presents
the most credible explanation for the phenomenon at hand. The two models employed
are, respectively, the Transformational (TG) and Feature Geometry (FG) models. It
appears that Feature Geometry model yields better results in the description of syllable
structure processes in Northern Sotho. / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Hierdie studie beskryf en verklaar sillabestruktuur prosesse in Noord-Sotho. Die
tersaaklike fonologiese prosesse is vokaaldelesie, semivokalisasie en
semivokaalinvoeging. Die doel van hierdie prosesse is om "n bepaalde
voorkeursillabestruktuur IKVI te herstel wat versteur word deur morfologiese prosesse met
die vorming van die passief, diminutief, die konstruksie van absolute voornaamwoorde
ensovoorts.
Twee fonologiese modelle word geïmplementeer ten einde te bepaal welke model die
mees geloofwaardige verklarings vir die betrokke verskynsels kan bied. Die
Transformasioneel-Generatiewe (TG) en Kenmerk Geometriese(KG) modelle word
respektiewelik toegepas. Dit skyn asof die Kenmerk Geometriese model beter resultate
lewer in die beskrywing van sillabestruktuurprosesse in Noord-Sotho.
|
487 |
Politeness theory and requests in XhosaDlali, Mawande,1965- 04 1900 (has links)
Thesis (PhD)--University of Stellenbosch, 2001. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: This study investigates how politeness may be employed in requests in Xhosa. While
numerous studies on speech act have been conducted in different languages, the
investigation of speech acts in African Languages, particularly Xhosa, shows no such
progress. This study attempts to fill this gap by examining the notions of politeness in
requests as perceived among the Xhosas.
With the study of speech acts, two instances of meaning have been identified. In the first
meaning a speaker utters a sentence and means exactly and literally what he says. In the
second meaning the speaker utters a sentence with an additional illocution with a different
prepositional content.
It has been established that various meanings playa role in the understanding of indirect
requests. This finding is based on the theory of Brown and Levinson's (1987) face work of
politeness. Scholars like Clark and Schunk (1980) argue that the politeness of response is
governed by the attentiveness hypothesis which states that the more attentive the hearer
is to all aspects of the speaker's request, within reason, the more polite he is.
One of the most common motivations for politeness is a request. Brown and Levinson
(1987) define politeness as the manifestation of respect for and recognition of another's
face. They delineate face into two components: negative face and positive face. Positive
face is the way a person wants to be regarded, admired, or approved by others and to be
treated as a friend. On the other hand, negative face is the person's desire not to be
imposed on by other people. A request threatens face in the sense that it imposes on the
hearer, that is why in some cases requests call for mitigation, so as to compensate for
their impositive effect on the hearer.
Various subcategories of requests within which negative politeness may appear have been
established, as well as the various ways in which these subcategories of requests may be
linguistically expressed in Xhosa. Three distribution types of request categories with
negative politeness have been found: high frequency, No very regular and negligible. Requests with the highest frequency may be divided into three subcategories: compliance;
information; and action. These subcategories demand non-threatening strategies:
compliance demands obedience, action demand doing things with a desired result, and
information demands knowledge from a person. These three are thus face-threatening
acts, which demand respect for the hearer's antonomy. Such requests may seriously
threaten the hearer's negative face. If no politeness strategy is attempted, these requests
will be viewed as most threatening acts.
The ten negative politeness strategies of Brown and Levinson did not apply to Xhosa
because they have been developed for a Western language. In the place of these
strategies, it has been found that negative politeness may be expressed in Xhosa through
certain subcategories as above, but also through certain pragmatic functions by means of
which negative politeness may be applied to avert a face-threatening act.
Brown and Levinson (1987) list fifteen positive politeness strategies. In the case of the
four Xhosa books, which were analyzed, only eight strategies for positive politeness were
found. These strategies can be divided into two groups: high frequency and negligible.
The most regular strategies are: seek agreement, give or ask for reasons, address forms,
presupposition, and those, which include both speaker and hearer.
An explanation for the high frequency of these strategies is to be found within positive
politeness. Positive politeness forms emphasize closeness between speaker and hearer
and it can be seen as a solidarity strategy. Thus, a face saving act, which is concerned
with the person's positive face, will show solidarity. / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Hierdie studie ondersoek hoe beleeftheid aangewend kan word in Xhosa versoeke. Daar is
verskeie studies oor spraakhandelinge in verskillende tale, maar die ondersoek na
spraakhandelinge in die Afrikatale, veral Xhosa, toon nie sodanige vooruitgang nie.
Hierdie studie poog om hierdie gaping te vul deur die begrip beleefdheid in versoeke by
die Xhosa te ondersoek.
In die studie van spraakhandelinge is twee instansies van betekenis geïdentifiseer. In die
eerste betekenis uiter 'n spreker 'n sin en die betekenis is presies wat gesê word. In die
tweede betekenis uiter die spreker 'n sin met 'n bykomende illokusie met 'n verskillende
proposisionele indruk.
Daar is vasgestel dat verskeie betekenisse 'n rol speel in die verstaan van indirekte
versoeke. Hierdie bevinding is gebaseer op die teorie van Brown en Levinson (1987) se
werk oor gesig in beleefdheid. Onder andere Clark en Schunk (1980) is van mening dat
die beleefdheid van 'n respons op 'n versoek beheer word deur die attentheid hipotese
waardeur aangedui word dat hoe meer aandagtig 'n hoorder is op alle aspekte van die
spreker se versoek, hoe meer beleefd hy is.
Een van die algemeenste motiverings vir beleefdheid is versoeke. Brown en Levinson
(1987) definieer beleefdheid as die manifestasie van respek en erkenning van 'n ander se
gesig. Hulle grens gesig af in twee dele: negatiewe gesig en positiewe gesig. Positiewe
gesig is die wyse waarop 'n persoon beskou, bewonder of waardeer word deur ander en
om soos 'n vriend behandel te word. Aan die ander kant, negatiewe gesig is 'n persoon se
begeerte om nie bedrieg te word deur ander mense. 'n Versoek bedreig gesig in die sin
dat dit misbruik maak van die hoorder. Dit is waarom in sommige gevalle daar versagting
vir versoeke is om te vergoed vir die misbruikmaking op die hoorder.
Verskeie subkategorieë van versoeke waarin negatiewe beleefdheid voorkom is onderskei,
asook die verskillende wyses waarin hierdie subkategorieë van versoeke linguisties
uitgedruk kan word in Xhosa. Drie spreidingstipes van versoek kategorieë met negatiewe
beleefdheid is gevind: hoë frekwensie, nie baie reëlmatig en onbeduidend. Versoeke met die hoogste frekwensie kan verdeel word in drie subkategorieë: inskiklikheid, inligting en
handeling. Hierdie subkategorieë vereis strategieë wat nie bedreiging inhou: inskiklikheid
vereis gehoorsaamheid, handeling vereis dat iets gedoen moet word met 'n sekere
resultaat, en inligting vereis kennis van 'n persoon. Hierdie drie is dus handelinge wat 'n
bedreiging inhou vir gesig en wat respek vereis vir die hoorder se outonomie. Sulke
versoeke kan 'n ernstige bedreiging inhou vir die hoorder se negatiewe gesig. As geen
beleefdheidsstrategie gevolg word, kan hierdie versoeke beskou word as handelinge wat
geweldig bedreigend is.
Die tien negatiewe beleefdheidsstrategieë van Brown en Levinson (1987) is nie van
toepassing in Xhosa nie, omdat dit ontwikkel is vir 'n Westerse taal. In die plek van
hierdie strategieë is gevind dat negatiewe beleefdheid in Xhosa uitgedruk kan word deur
sekere subkategorieë soos hierbo, maar ook deur sekere pragmatiese funksies waardeur
negatiewe beleefdheid aangewend kan word om in handeling wat gesig bedreig af te
weer.
Brown en Levinson (1987) gee 15 strategieë vir positiewe beleefdheid. In die geval van
Xhosa is agt (8) van hierdie strategieë gevind in die boeke wat geanaliseer is. Hierdie
strategieë kan in twee groepe verdeel word: hoë frekwensie en onbeduidend. Die
strategieë met die hoogste frekwensie is: soek ooreenstemming; gee of vra redes;
aanspreekvorme, presupposisieen die wat beide spreker en hoorder insluit.
'n Verklaring vir die hoë frekwensie van hierdie strategieë kan gevind word in positiewe
beleefdheid. Positiewe beleefdheid benadruk nabyheid tussen spreker en hoorder en dit
kan gesien word as 'n solidariteitsstrategie. Dus, 'n handeling wat gesig red wat betrokke
is by'n persoon se positiewe gesig sal solidariteit toon.
|
488 |
Copulative verbs in Northern Sotho :a morphosemantic studyMaseko, Julia Refilwe January 2005 (has links)
Thesis (M. A. (African Languages) --University of Limpopo,2005 / The study aimsat finding out thecategorical status of copulatives in Northern Sotho. This will be achieved by examining the morphosemantic features of various copulatives. From a morphological perspective, thestudy focuses on the following types ofcopulative verbs: ke, se, ba, le, na, and COP. The study argues that the foresaid copulatives are not particles but are fully-fledged verbs.
As far as the semantic nature of the copulatives is concerned, the study discovered a variety of meanings associated with copulatives, such as the following: the identifying, descriptive,locational and associative.
Lastly, the study contends that any research on the copulative in Northern Sotho should be a morphosemantic one, as previous studies focused on one and neglected the other.
|
489 |
Utterance particles in Taiwanese conversation and their pragmatic functionsHuang, Mei-Chu S. January 2002 (has links)
The discourse functions of three Taiwanese particles la, hon and ne are analyzed. These utterance particles occur primarily found in the spoken language, usually conversation. The corpus of the study comprises transcription of recorded face-to-face, telephone, and radio talk show conversation, casual interviews, and church sermons. All three Taiwanese particles express a form of emphasis but in different ways. La has five interrelated functions, depending on the context: 1. To express in an assertion and in an answer to a question; 2. To express a sense of guessing or questioning; 3. To express one's impatience, dislike, or annoyance; 4. To indicate a sense of coaxing; 5. To emphasize each item in an incomplete list. Hon expresses the lowest degree of speaker emphasis of the three particles. It is used to elicit a minimal or a positive response from the addressee. Finally, ne expresses the strongest degree of emphasis of the three. It is usually used to draw the addressee's attention to information that the speaker assumes is new to the hearer. / Department of English
|
490 |
Case absorption and WH-agreement /Watanabe, Akira. January 1996 (has links)
1993--Diss., 1993.
|
Page generated in 0.1012 seconds