Spelling suggestions: "subject:"schoolaged children"" "subject:"schooledaged children""
31 |
An assets based approach to health promotion with young people in EnglandFenton, Catherine Meghan January 2013 (has links)
Introduction The health of young people in England is an area of concern, nationally and internationally. This has prompted a range of strategies and policies to try to address how health may be improved. However, there has not necessarily been agreement as to how this should best be done. There appeared a case for consideration of alternative or additional approaches to health promotion. Aim This research aimed to construct an assets based model to shape health promotion practice and policy for young people in England. Methods A narrative synthesis was undertaken and highlighted the lack of information regarding which assets might be important for young people’s health in England. This programme of research was developed from those initial findings. Quantitative and qualitative methods were employed to gain a more comprehensive understanding than could be gained by individual methods. This mixed methods research involved secondary data analysis of the Health Behaviours in School Aged Children (HBSC) dataset using regression analysis to identify the assets associated with life satisfaction for English youth. Focus groups and interviews were employed to capture the views of young people regarding assets, health and health promotion. Findings were discussed with practitioners to gather their ideas as to the potential of an assets approach. The different research methods were drawn together by the underpinning theoretical frameworks provided by Assets models and the New Social Studies of Childhood. Results Two themes emerged from the narrative synthesis providing suggestions for health promotion; the ecological approach acknowledged the range of settings that young people inhabit, whilst the holistic approach recognised the interrelationship between risks and assets. Critical discussion consolidated the research findings to propose a list of health promoting assets for young people in England; constructive relationships, safety, positive attributes, independence and opportunity. These findings were brought together into a descriptive model to guide health promotion policy and practice (Figure 1). Constructive relationships appeared as a core asset, providing a foundation from which young people could develop. Having positive attributes was also fundamental to this process, which emphasises the importance of promoting physical and mental health simultaneously. Safety was the third core asset identified through the research strands. There was variation between young people regarding the definition of, and priority assigned to, the additional assets of independence and opportunity. Figure 1: Assets model to shape health promotion with young people Conclusion This research contributes to previous work in the field of assets models by providing new insight into the relatively little researched area of assets approaches to health promotion with young people in England. The complex interrelationships between mental health and assets have been highlighted; providing challenge to frameworks that focus on the quantitative accumulation of assets. The inclusion of young people’s perspectives provided new depth to previous theoretical models and interpretation of quantitative findings. The variation highlighted within this research raises implications for tackling health inequalities. This assets based model provides a framework to shape professional practice and policy thus providing the potential to improve young people’s health and wellbeing in a sustainable and non-stigmatising way.
|
32 |
Fonctionnement auditif central d’enfants ayant une surditéKoravand, Amineh 08 1900 (has links)
La présente recherche explore les conséquences d’une perte auditive périphérique sur le traitement de l’information auditive. Des études ont montré que les enfants malentendants ont de la difficulté à effectuer des tâches d’écoute complexes. De plus, des études menées auprès d’adultes malentendants montrent que l’activité corticale associée à l’écoute de stimuli auditifs est différente de celle d’adultes entendants. Cependant, les résultats de ces études ne mettent pas en lumière la nature des difficultés de traitement de l’information auditive des enfants malentendants. Cette recherche examine donc cet aspect en ayant recours à des mesures comportementales et neurophysiologiques.
Les données ont été recueillies auprès de 40 enfants âgés de 9 à 12 ans : 12 enfants ayant une surdité neurosensorielle, 12 enfants ayant trouble de traitement auditif et 16 enfants normo-entendants. Les enfants ont reproduit dans l’ordre des séquences de deux, trois et cinq stimuli verbaux ou non verbaux avec un intervalle interstimuli de 425 ms. Les enfants ont également reproduit des séquences de deux stimuli avec un intervalle interstimuli de 20 et 1000 ms. Enfin, les enfants ont été soumis à des mesures neurophysiologiques à partir de potentiels évoqués auditifs de latence longue et de négativité de discordance avec des paires de stimuli verbaux et non verbaux.
Les résultats obtenus permettent d’avancer que les participants du groupe d’enfants malentendants ont un trouble spécifique de traitement auditif. En effet, les résultats de la tâche comportementale montrent que les enfants malentendants ont de la difficulté à traiter des séquences de stimuli lorsque ceux-ci sont verbaux et acoustiquement similaires. Quant aux données neurophysiologiques, les résultats ont démontré que l’amplitude de l’onde tardive N2 était réduite chez les enfants malentendants comparativement à celle de l’onde N2 des deux autres groupes d’enfants. Cette onde pourrait être considérée comme étant un marqueur neurophysiologique reflétant l’influence d’une perte auditive sur le traitement auditif central. De plus, l’amplitude de l’onde de négativité de discordance pourrait être aussi un marqueur pour distinguer les enfants malentendants de ceux ayant un trouble de traitement auditif.Mots-clés : organisation séquentielle auditive, potentiels évoqués auditifs de latence longue, négativité de discordance, enfants malentendants d’âge scolaire / The present research explores the effects of peripheral hearing loss on central auditory processing. Previous studies showed that children with hearing loss have significant difficulties in performing complex listening tasks. Moreover, studies of adults with hearing loss revealed that cortical activity related to listening to acoustic stimuli differed from that of adults without hearing loss. However, results of these studies do not clarify the nature of the difficulties in processing auditory information among children with hearing loss. The present research examines this issue using behavioural and neurophysiological measures.
Behavioural and neurophysiological data were collected with forty 9- to 12-year-old children: 12 with hearing loss, 12 with central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) and 16 with normal hearing. Children repeated, in order, two, three, and five verbal and nonverbal stimuli with an interstimulus interval of 425 ms. They also repeated sequences of two stimuli with an interstimulus interval of 20 or 1000 ms. Finally, late-latency auditory evoked potentials and mismatch responses were recorded in the participants using pairs of verbal and nonverbal stimuli.
Results suggest that children with hearing loss have a specific central auditory processing disorder. Results of the behavioural task showed that children with hearing loss had difficulty processing sequences of stimuli when the stimuli were verbal as well as similar and complex acoustically. As for the neurophysiological data, results indicated that the amplitude of late N2 waveform was smaller in children with hearing loss than in the other two groups of children. The N2 waveform has the potential to be a neurophysiological marker revealing the influence of hearing loss on central auditory processing. Moreover, the amplitude of the mismatch response could be another marker to distinguish the children with hearing loss from those with central auditory processing disorder.
Keywords : Auditory sequential organization, late-latency auditory evoked potentials, mismatch responses, school-aged children with hearing loss
|
33 |
Fonctionnement auditif central d’enfants ayant une surditéKoravand, Amineh 08 1900 (has links)
La présente recherche explore les conséquences d’une perte auditive périphérique sur le traitement de l’information auditive. Des études ont montré que les enfants malentendants ont de la difficulté à effectuer des tâches d’écoute complexes. De plus, des études menées auprès d’adultes malentendants montrent que l’activité corticale associée à l’écoute de stimuli auditifs est différente de celle d’adultes entendants. Cependant, les résultats de ces études ne mettent pas en lumière la nature des difficultés de traitement de l’information auditive des enfants malentendants. Cette recherche examine donc cet aspect en ayant recours à des mesures comportementales et neurophysiologiques.
Les données ont été recueillies auprès de 40 enfants âgés de 9 à 12 ans : 12 enfants ayant une surdité neurosensorielle, 12 enfants ayant trouble de traitement auditif et 16 enfants normo-entendants. Les enfants ont reproduit dans l’ordre des séquences de deux, trois et cinq stimuli verbaux ou non verbaux avec un intervalle interstimuli de 425 ms. Les enfants ont également reproduit des séquences de deux stimuli avec un intervalle interstimuli de 20 et 1000 ms. Enfin, les enfants ont été soumis à des mesures neurophysiologiques à partir de potentiels évoqués auditifs de latence longue et de négativité de discordance avec des paires de stimuli verbaux et non verbaux.
Les résultats obtenus permettent d’avancer que les participants du groupe d’enfants malentendants ont un trouble spécifique de traitement auditif. En effet, les résultats de la tâche comportementale montrent que les enfants malentendants ont de la difficulté à traiter des séquences de stimuli lorsque ceux-ci sont verbaux et acoustiquement similaires. Quant aux données neurophysiologiques, les résultats ont démontré que l’amplitude de l’onde tardive N2 était réduite chez les enfants malentendants comparativement à celle de l’onde N2 des deux autres groupes d’enfants. Cette onde pourrait être considérée comme étant un marqueur neurophysiologique reflétant l’influence d’une perte auditive sur le traitement auditif central. De plus, l’amplitude de l’onde de négativité de discordance pourrait être aussi un marqueur pour distinguer les enfants malentendants de ceux ayant un trouble de traitement auditif.Mots-clés : organisation séquentielle auditive, potentiels évoqués auditifs de latence longue, négativité de discordance, enfants malentendants d’âge scolaire / The present research explores the effects of peripheral hearing loss on central auditory processing. Previous studies showed that children with hearing loss have significant difficulties in performing complex listening tasks. Moreover, studies of adults with hearing loss revealed that cortical activity related to listening to acoustic stimuli differed from that of adults without hearing loss. However, results of these studies do not clarify the nature of the difficulties in processing auditory information among children with hearing loss. The present research examines this issue using behavioural and neurophysiological measures.
Behavioural and neurophysiological data were collected with forty 9- to 12-year-old children: 12 with hearing loss, 12 with central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) and 16 with normal hearing. Children repeated, in order, two, three, and five verbal and nonverbal stimuli with an interstimulus interval of 425 ms. They also repeated sequences of two stimuli with an interstimulus interval of 20 or 1000 ms. Finally, late-latency auditory evoked potentials and mismatch responses were recorded in the participants using pairs of verbal and nonverbal stimuli.
Results suggest that children with hearing loss have a specific central auditory processing disorder. Results of the behavioural task showed that children with hearing loss had difficulty processing sequences of stimuli when the stimuli were verbal as well as similar and complex acoustically. As for the neurophysiological data, results indicated that the amplitude of late N2 waveform was smaller in children with hearing loss than in the other two groups of children. The N2 waveform has the potential to be a neurophysiological marker revealing the influence of hearing loss on central auditory processing. Moreover, the amplitude of the mismatch response could be another marker to distinguish the children with hearing loss from those with central auditory processing disorder.
Keywords : Auditory sequential organization, late-latency auditory evoked potentials, mismatch responses, school-aged children with hearing loss
|
34 |
Dynamic assessment i svenskspråkig kontext : En explorativ studie av sex-sjuåringars narrativa utvecklingKristoffersson, Louise January 2011 (has links)
ABSTRACT In standardized tests, culturally and linguistically diverse (CLD) children usually perform at similar level as children with language impairment (LI), largely due to the fact that most standardized assessment tools have been developed on the basis of a monolingual population with mainstream backgrounds. As an alternative to standardized tests, a dynamic assessment approach has been advocated to assess the language abilities of CLD children. Dynamic assessment is considered to reduce test bias, since it measures the child's learning potential (i.e. modifiability) instead of the child's static performance on a certain task at one point in time. The aim of the present study was to explore if dynamic assessment was as successful in a Swedish context as it has been in American contexts. The study had a test-teach-retest design with every session conducted individually. Five typically developing monolingual Swedish children, aged six to seven, were asked to tell two stories on the basis of two comparable picture sequences. In between, a teaching session, that consisted of a mediated learning experience (MLE), was conducted. During the MLE session, a certain aspect of storytelling, chosen from the analysis of the test narrative, was targeted and 5 trained. After the MLE session, the child's modifiability was measured using five-point scales for teaching effort and student responsiveness. The test and retest results were then compared. The DA results indicated that three of the children were typically developing and that two of the children, contrary to fact, had some kind of language learning disability. The material used was found to have limitations in its narrow rating criteria and complicated scripts for the MLE sessions. The conclusions were that (a) the assessment was influenced by the examiner's subjective rating and therefore unable to show the children's true narrative abilities, (b) that the MLE material was not fully suited for younger school aged children and (c) not suitable in the Swedish context. Further studies have to be made with a larger number of children, including CLD children, to be able to tell whether dynamic assessment is a suitable method in Swedish contexts or not. A special DA material for Swedish assessments would then be useful. / Typiskt utvecklade barn med atypisk kulturell bakgrund, flerspråkighet eller låg socioekonomisk status (dvs. barn med språkskillnad) som genomgår språktestning med standardiserade tester förefaller prestera lika på dessa som barn med språkstörning. Detta pga. att de flesta standardiserade tester är skapade för enspråkiga barn med typisk bakgrund. Barnen med språkskillnad riskerar således att bli feldiagnostiserade med språkstörning. Dynamic assessment (DA) har i främst amerikanska studier visat sig vara en fördelaktig bedömningsmetod vid sådana fall, då den har lyckats särskilja barn med språkskillnad från barn med språkstörning. Detta eftersom DA, till skillnad från standardiserade tester, inte undersöker barnets förmåga att klara en viss uppgift vid ett tillfälle, utan istället undersöker barnets förmåga att lära sig att klara av en uppgift (dvs. barnets modifierbarhet). Föreliggande studie avsåg att undersöka hur DA av narrativ förmåga fungerade med svenskspråkiga typiskt utvecklade barn i åldern sex-sju år. Deltagarna genomgick individuell DA med test-teach-retest-design och fick först berätta en saga till en bildsekvens. En aspekt av narrativ förmåga, t.ex. kausalitet, valdes sedan ut och tränades vid en undervisningssession som utgjordes av en så kallad mediated learning experience (MLE). Barnets modifierbarhet under MLE skattades med hjälp av femgradiga skalor för läraransträngning och barnets responsivitet. Därefter följde retest, där varje barn fick berätta en saga till en ny bildsekvens. En jämförelse mellan test och retest skedde. Resultaten visade att tre av deltagarna fick de resultat som förväntats av barn med typisk utveckling. Två deltagare fick resultat som indikerade språkstörning. Materialet som användes bedömdes ha snävt satta bedömningskriterier och svåranvända skript för undervisningssessionerna. Slutsatserna som drogs var (a) att DA-materialet som användes gav en mycket subjektiv bedömning som inte stämde överens med barnets faktiska narrativa förmåga, (b) att MLE-skripten inte var designade på ett helt barnvänligt sätt och (c) inte passade i svensk kontext. Fler och större studier behöver göras för att kunna ge en tydligare bild av hur DA fungerar i svensk kontext. I detta fall vore ett svenskt DA-material önskvärt.
|
35 |
Expressivt ordförråd hos skolbarn med lätt till måttligt svår hörselnedsättningEdquist, Gertrud January 2021 (has links)
Abstract Children with hearing disorders face particular challenges in developing their vocabulary. They often have difficulty in acquiring spoken language at the same rate as their normal hearing peers, despite progress regarding hearing aid technology.Aim. The main aim of this study was to describe aspects of expressive vocabulary for school-age children with mild to moderately severe hearing impairment (HI-group), and to compare with results for a group of normal hearing children (NH-group) of the same age. One aim was also to test whether there were differences, between children with hearing impairment and normal hearing children, in the ability to repeat non-words and sentences and to compare these results within each group with results on two newly constructed vocabulary tests. An additional aim was also to investigate whether background factors, such as age of diagnosis, age of hearing aid fitting and amount of hearing aid use, were related to results on vocabulary tests.Method: 24 children age 7-11 with mild to moderately severe bilateral hearing impairment, and 82 normal hearing children of similar age, were included in this cross-sectional study. Assessments of picture naming, word definition, repetition of nonwords and repetition of sentences were performed with all participants. Age, hearing, use of hearing aid and parents' level of education were included in the analyzes.Results: The HI-group performed significantly below the results of the NH-group regarding picture naming, word definition, nonword repetition and repetition of sentences. The HI-group also produced significantly more words with deficiencies in phonological structure, than the NH-group, during picture naming. In the HI-group there was a significant positive correlation between amount of hearing aid use per day and nonword repetition. Age of diagnose and age of hearing aid fitting did not show statistically significant associations with outcomes on the language tests in this study.Conclusion: Despite the technical development of hearing aids, the expressive vocabulary in school-aged children with bilateral, mild to moderately severe, hearing impairment, does not reach the same level as in normal-hearing peers, although there is a variation within the group. Amount of hearing aid use seems to be important for the ability to perceive new words.
|
36 |
School Support Staff and Student Outcomes in Large Urban Districts in the Midwest A Correlational StudyBirkhimer, Courtney B. 03 May 2022 (has links)
No description available.
|
37 |
Développement des habiletés linguistiques chez les enfants porteurs d'un implant cochléaireDuchesne, Louise 12 1900 (has links)
Cette thèse examine le développement du langage des enfants sourds qui ont reçu un implant cochléaire (IC) en bas âge. Une première étude rapporte une revue systématique qui avait pour but d’évaluer les connaissances actuelles concernant le développement du vocabulaire et de la grammaire chez les enfants qui ont reçu un IC avant l’âge de trois ans. Vingt-huit études ont été sélectionnées; une analyse descriptive de même qu’une méta-analyse ont été effectuées séparément pour chaque aspect du langage évalué (vocabulaire et grammaire, aspect réceptif et expressif). Au résultat, en dépit de la variabilité observée dans les études, il appert que l’implant cochléaire influence positivement le développement langagier; toutefois, seule une minorité de participants aux études a atteint des niveaux de langage comparables à ceux d’enfants entendants de même âge chronologique. La majorité des enfants continuent de présenter divers degrés de retard de langage, tant au plan réceptif qu’expressif, et ce, après jusqu’à cinq années de port de l’appareil. Les résultats suggèrent aussi, malgré la variabilité observée dans les études, que les bénéfices langagiers sont influencés par le fait de recevoir l’implant à deux ans plutôt qu’à trois ans.
À partir des tendances retrouvées dans la littérature, les habiletés de vocabulaire et de grammaire chez 27 enfants qui ont reçu l’implant cochléaire en bas âge (entre 8 et 28 mois) ont été comparées avec celles d’un groupe d’enfants entendants, en utilisant des outils d’évaluation standardisés. Alors que les résultats de groupe montrent que les enfants qui reçoivent un IC autour de l’âge de deux ans atteignent des niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale, les résultats individuels d’un sous-groupe formé de enfants les plus âgés font état de quatre profils de développement, soit des niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale pour l’ensemble des composantes, un retard généralisé à l’ensemble des composantes, des habiletés lexicales dans la norme assorti d’un retard morphosyntaxique et enfin un profil atypique montrant des disparités importantes à travers les composantes du langage. Dans trois des quatre profils, la compréhension des phrases était particulièrement faible. Ces résultats suggèrent que le fait de recevoir un implant cochléaire entre l’âge d’un et deux ans ne garantit pas l’atteinte de niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale malgré une expérience de port de l’appareil d’une durée appréciable.
Une étape antérieure du développement linguistique a été examinée de plus près dans la troisième étude. La taille et la composition du vocabulaire expressif de onze enfants ayant reçu un IC à un âge moyen de 15 mois ont été comparées à celles de l’échantillon d’enfants entendants ayant servi à établir les normes en français québécois pour le questionnaire Mots et énoncés des Inventaires MacArthur-Bates du développement de la communication (IMBDC). Les scores d’âge équivalent selon la taille totale du vocabulaire des enfants avec IC étaient supérieurs à l’âge auditif (correspondant à la durée de port de l’appareil) mais inférieurs à l’âge chronologique. La représentation grammaticale en fonction de la taille du vocabulaire des enfants avec IC suit la tendance observée dans la norme. Ces résultats suggèrent que le profil lexical des enfants avec implant est très similaire à celui des enfants entendants lorsque le nombre total de mots acquis est le même.
Les résultats de cette thèse suggèrent que l’implant peut, de manière générale, avoir un effet « normalisant » sur le langage ; toutefois, il semble que l’amélioration de l’accès auditif ne suffise pas pour rattraper à coup sûr le niveau de langage des pairs entendants dans l’ensemble des composantes du langage. Alors que les habiletés lexicales se rapprochent du profil typique, les habiletés de compréhension morphosyntaxique sont fortement atteintes chez une majorité d’enfants, suggérant un profil apparenté à un trouble de langage. / This dissertation examines language development in children who received a cochlear implant (CI) at a young age. A systematic review studied the main outcomes reported in the literature concerning vocabulary and grammar development of profoundly deaf children who received a CI before the age of 3 years. A total of 28 studies met inclusion criteria. Studies varied widely in participant characteristics, assessment tools, study designs, and overall methodological quality. A descriptive synthesis and a meta-analysis were completed separately for each language domain (receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar). Results showed a positive impact of cochlear implantation on language development, but only a minority of children achieved language levels on par with their hearing age-mates. The majority of the children involved in the primary studies continued to exhibit varying levels of delay in receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar after up to 5 years of cochlear implant use. Results showed that the minority of children who achieved language levels on par with hearing children were likely to have received their implant by the age of two, thus suggesting that age at implantation influences language achievement.
A second study examined receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar achievement of 27 French-speaking children who received a CI between the age of 8 and 28 months. Standardized measures were administered and the language levels attained by children with CIs were compared with those of the normative sample of same-age hearing peers for each measure. As a group, children exhibited language levels within normal limits on all standardized language measures. Examination of individual patterns in a subgroup of children revealed 4 different language profiles: a) normal language levels in all domains, b) general language delay, c) vocabulary within the norm with morphosyntactic delay, and d) an atypical profile (discrepancies across language domains). In three of these profiles, comprehension of sentences was impaired. Findings suggest that receiving a cochlear implant between the age of 1 and 2 years does not ensure that language abilities will be within normal limits after up to 6 years of experience with the implant.
An earlier stage of formal language was examined in the third study. Vocabulary size and grammatical composition in 11 children who received their CI at a mean age of 15 months were compared to that of the Quebec French normative sample for the Words and Sentences questionnaire of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MBCDI). Results showed that age equivalent scores according to total vocabulary size were superior to hearing age (equivalent to the duration of device use) but inferior to chronological age. Distribution of grammatical categories according to vocabulary size followed the same pattern as in the normative sample. These results suggest that the lexical profile of children with implants was very similar to that of normally-hearing children who had the same number of words.
Taken together, results of this dissertation suggest that the cochlear implant can have a “normalizing” effect on language. However, they also suggest that improved access to auditory input does not seem sufficient to allow children to attain language levels within normal limits in all components. Whereas early lexical abilities were comparable to typical development, receptive morphosyntactic abilities remain severely impaired in a majority of children.
|
38 |
Assessing Teachers' Confidence in Implementing Food Allergy Emergency PlansHawkins, Keturah Elizabeth 01 January 2017 (has links)
Food allergies are an increasing health concern in the United States, affecting nearly 6 million children under the age of 18 years. Research has suggested that 18% of school-age children will have their first allergic reactions at school. Life-threatening allergic reactions experienced by children in the school setting are on the rise; however, little is known about how schools implement policies and practices in response to this issue. The purpose of this quantitative cross-sectional study was to narrow the knowledge gap by examining teachers' knowledge, ability, and confidence level caring for students with food allergies. Bandura's social cognitive theory, which holds that education and experience influence confidence implementing tasks, served as the framework that guided this research. The electronic survey was distributed to a convenience sample of 300 elementary school teachers; 93 respondents completed it. Eighty completed surveys were used in the analysis. Multiple linear regression models were constructed to analyze the relationships among confidence, education, and training related to food allergies. Results showed that teachers who lacked knowledge of food allergies also lacked confidence implementing food allergy plans. School personnel responsible for planning or revising food allergy response protocols can use these findings. The potential for positive social change includes identifying training opportunities, developing policies to sustain food allergy knowledge, and building the capacity of all school staff to implement life-saving measures when children are experiencing allergic reactions.
|
39 |
Développement des habiletés linguistiques chez les enfants porteurs d'un implant cochléaireDuchesne, Louise 12 1900 (has links)
Cette thèse examine le développement du langage des enfants sourds qui ont reçu un implant cochléaire (IC) en bas âge. Une première étude rapporte une revue systématique qui avait pour but d’évaluer les connaissances actuelles concernant le développement du vocabulaire et de la grammaire chez les enfants qui ont reçu un IC avant l’âge de trois ans. Vingt-huit études ont été sélectionnées; une analyse descriptive de même qu’une méta-analyse ont été effectuées séparément pour chaque aspect du langage évalué (vocabulaire et grammaire, aspect réceptif et expressif). Au résultat, en dépit de la variabilité observée dans les études, il appert que l’implant cochléaire influence positivement le développement langagier; toutefois, seule une minorité de participants aux études a atteint des niveaux de langage comparables à ceux d’enfants entendants de même âge chronologique. La majorité des enfants continuent de présenter divers degrés de retard de langage, tant au plan réceptif qu’expressif, et ce, après jusqu’à cinq années de port de l’appareil. Les résultats suggèrent aussi, malgré la variabilité observée dans les études, que les bénéfices langagiers sont influencés par le fait de recevoir l’implant à deux ans plutôt qu’à trois ans.
À partir des tendances retrouvées dans la littérature, les habiletés de vocabulaire et de grammaire chez 27 enfants qui ont reçu l’implant cochléaire en bas âge (entre 8 et 28 mois) ont été comparées avec celles d’un groupe d’enfants entendants, en utilisant des outils d’évaluation standardisés. Alors que les résultats de groupe montrent que les enfants qui reçoivent un IC autour de l’âge de deux ans atteignent des niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale, les résultats individuels d’un sous-groupe formé de enfants les plus âgés font état de quatre profils de développement, soit des niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale pour l’ensemble des composantes, un retard généralisé à l’ensemble des composantes, des habiletés lexicales dans la norme assorti d’un retard morphosyntaxique et enfin un profil atypique montrant des disparités importantes à travers les composantes du langage. Dans trois des quatre profils, la compréhension des phrases était particulièrement faible. Ces résultats suggèrent que le fait de recevoir un implant cochléaire entre l’âge d’un et deux ans ne garantit pas l’atteinte de niveaux de langage dans les limites de la normale malgré une expérience de port de l’appareil d’une durée appréciable.
Une étape antérieure du développement linguistique a été examinée de plus près dans la troisième étude. La taille et la composition du vocabulaire expressif de onze enfants ayant reçu un IC à un âge moyen de 15 mois ont été comparées à celles de l’échantillon d’enfants entendants ayant servi à établir les normes en français québécois pour le questionnaire Mots et énoncés des Inventaires MacArthur-Bates du développement de la communication (IMBDC). Les scores d’âge équivalent selon la taille totale du vocabulaire des enfants avec IC étaient supérieurs à l’âge auditif (correspondant à la durée de port de l’appareil) mais inférieurs à l’âge chronologique. La représentation grammaticale en fonction de la taille du vocabulaire des enfants avec IC suit la tendance observée dans la norme. Ces résultats suggèrent que le profil lexical des enfants avec implant est très similaire à celui des enfants entendants lorsque le nombre total de mots acquis est le même.
Les résultats de cette thèse suggèrent que l’implant peut, de manière générale, avoir un effet « normalisant » sur le langage ; toutefois, il semble que l’amélioration de l’accès auditif ne suffise pas pour rattraper à coup sûr le niveau de langage des pairs entendants dans l’ensemble des composantes du langage. Alors que les habiletés lexicales se rapprochent du profil typique, les habiletés de compréhension morphosyntaxique sont fortement atteintes chez une majorité d’enfants, suggérant un profil apparenté à un trouble de langage. / This dissertation examines language development in children who received a cochlear implant (CI) at a young age. A systematic review studied the main outcomes reported in the literature concerning vocabulary and grammar development of profoundly deaf children who received a CI before the age of 3 years. A total of 28 studies met inclusion criteria. Studies varied widely in participant characteristics, assessment tools, study designs, and overall methodological quality. A descriptive synthesis and a meta-analysis were completed separately for each language domain (receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar). Results showed a positive impact of cochlear implantation on language development, but only a minority of children achieved language levels on par with their hearing age-mates. The majority of the children involved in the primary studies continued to exhibit varying levels of delay in receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar after up to 5 years of cochlear implant use. Results showed that the minority of children who achieved language levels on par with hearing children were likely to have received their implant by the age of two, thus suggesting that age at implantation influences language achievement.
A second study examined receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar achievement of 27 French-speaking children who received a CI between the age of 8 and 28 months. Standardized measures were administered and the language levels attained by children with CIs were compared with those of the normative sample of same-age hearing peers for each measure. As a group, children exhibited language levels within normal limits on all standardized language measures. Examination of individual patterns in a subgroup of children revealed 4 different language profiles: a) normal language levels in all domains, b) general language delay, c) vocabulary within the norm with morphosyntactic delay, and d) an atypical profile (discrepancies across language domains). In three of these profiles, comprehension of sentences was impaired. Findings suggest that receiving a cochlear implant between the age of 1 and 2 years does not ensure that language abilities will be within normal limits after up to 6 years of experience with the implant.
An earlier stage of formal language was examined in the third study. Vocabulary size and grammatical composition in 11 children who received their CI at a mean age of 15 months were compared to that of the Quebec French normative sample for the Words and Sentences questionnaire of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MBCDI). Results showed that age equivalent scores according to total vocabulary size were superior to hearing age (equivalent to the duration of device use) but inferior to chronological age. Distribution of grammatical categories according to vocabulary size followed the same pattern as in the normative sample. These results suggest that the lexical profile of children with implants was very similar to that of normally-hearing children who had the same number of words.
Taken together, results of this dissertation suggest that the cochlear implant can have a “normalizing” effect on language. However, they also suggest that improved access to auditory input does not seem sufficient to allow children to attain language levels within normal limits in all components. Whereas early lexical abilities were comparable to typical development, receptive morphosyntactic abilities remain severely impaired in a majority of children.
|
Page generated in 0.0583 seconds