• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 253
  • 14
  • 12
  • 12
  • 12
  • 12
  • 12
  • 12
  • 9
  • 7
  • 7
  • 3
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 385
  • 385
  • 89
  • 80
  • 74
  • 68
  • 66
  • 62
  • 44
  • 40
  • 39
  • 38
  • 37
  • 37
  • 37
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
111

Selection, development and analysis of a test instrument in critical thinking for children in grades three, four and five

O'Sullivan, Ellen P. January 1973 (has links)
The underlying purpose of the study was to learn more about how elementary-aged children deal with tests purporting to measure critical thinking skills. This involved four related purposes: (a) development of a testing instrument, (b) analysis and evaluation of the test instrument, (c) to determine the difference of performance between grades, and (d) identification of commonality factors among the tests.
112

Emotion, thought, and therapy : a study of Hume, Spinoza and Freud on thought and passion

Neu, Jerome January 1974 (has links)
Hume and Spinoza are the most systematic representatives of two opposing traditions of argument about the relation of thought and feeling in the emotions. The Humeans treat emotions as essentially feelings (impressions or affects) with thoughts incidentally attached. The Spinozists say roughly the reverse, treating emotions as essentially thoughts ('ideas' or 'beliefs') with feelings incidentally attached. It is argued that the Spinozists are closer to the truth, that is, that thoughts are of greater importance than feelings fin the narrow sense of felt sensations) in the classification and discrimination of emotional states. It is then argued that if the Spinozists are closer to the truth, we have the beginning of an argument to show that Freudian or, more generally, analytic therapies make philosophic sense. That is, we can begin to understand how people's emotional lives might be trans- formed by consideration and interpretation of their memories, beliefs, etc.; how knowledge might help make one free.
113

Picture, process, and pattern :

Gold, Ian. January 1987 (has links)
No description available.
114

The emergence of the representational mind

Walker, Rebecca, n/a January 2006 (has links)
Theory of mind has been described in philosophical and psychological literature as "folk psychology", and is the tacit understanding that our behaviour is driven by our thoughts, desires and beliefs (Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001). Children are widely considered to have attained theory of mind understanding when they are able to pass the test of false belief understanding devised by Wimmer and Perner (1983), at around 4 years of age. There are many theories as to how a child comes to hold a folk psychology, including innate modularism (Leslie, 1987, 1988, 1994), theory change (Gopnik & Wellman 1992), developing representational understanding (Perner, 1991, 1995, 2000), and experiential understanding developed in a socio-linguisitic context (Nelson, 1996). In addition, theory of mind has been linked to the development of symbolic understanding (Deloache & Smith, 1999; Perner, 1991), pretend play (Leslie, 1987; Taylor & Carlson, 1997; Youngblade & Dunn, 1993), language (Astington & Jenkins, 1999; Nelson, 1996; Olson, 1988) and executive function (e.g. Hughes, 1998a; Kochanska et al., 1996; Reed et al., 1984). The present study sought to bring together these diverse findings and to attempt to provide an integrated account of the emergence of theory of mind understanding during the preschool years. Sixty-four New Zealand children were assessed on their mental state understanding, deceptive abilities, symbolic functioning, language, and executive skills, when they were aged 30, 36, 42 and 48 months of age. There were a number of key findings in the present study. Language was a powerful predictor of false belief performance both within and across time, and was also related to many of the other variables included in the study. Performance on the scale model test of symbolic functioning was related across time to children�s concurrent and later false belief understanding. Scale model performance was also intertwined in a bidirectional relationship with language, and language appeared to play an increasingly important role in mediating the relationship with false belief understanding across time. False belief understanding and scale model performance were also related within and across time to executive function. There was evidence to suggest that the importance of working memory was due to its role in conflict inhibition. Although deception has sometimes been posited to be a precocious manifestation of theory of mind (Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989), in the present study deceptive ability lagged false belief understanding. Furthermore, false belief understanding was related to children�s subsequent (but not earlier) responses to a protagonist�s intention. This supports the hypothesis that false belief understanding allows a qualitative change in the execution of deception, whereby children can move from simple physical strategies to more sophisticated mentalist strategies. Overall, the present study provides some evidence to suggest that symbolic functioning, language, and later theory of mind may form part of a single developing skill set of symbolic representation. In dynamic interaction with social understanding, and supported by cognitive abilities such as executive function, and the socio-linguistic context, it is argued that understanding of one�s own and other minds emerges. Children�s ability to solve the false belief problem at 4 years of age is presented as a milestone on a developmental continuum of social understanding.
115

An investigation of a new approach to teaching and learning designed to focus teachers and students on their thinking /

McGrath, Christopher William January 1999 (has links)
Thesis (PhD) -- University of South Australia, 1999
116

Human concept cognition and semantic relations in the unified medical language system a coherence analysis /

Assefa, Shimelis G. O'Connor, Brian C., January 2007 (has links)
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of North Texas, Aug., 2007. / Title from title page display. Includes bibliographical references.
117

The relationship between thinking dispositions, epistemic beliefs, and disjunctive reasoning.

Potworowski, Georges A. January 2004 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--University of Toronto, 2004. / Advisers: Keith E. Stanovich; Michel Ferrari.
118

The rate of inner speech /

Brock, Mary Angela, January 1900 (has links)
Thesis (M.S.)--Missouri State University, 2008. / "May 2008." Includes bibliographical references (leaves 17-18). Also available online.
119

Manipulation of cognitive biases and rumination an examination of single and combined correction conditions /

Adler, Abby Danielle, January 2008 (has links)
Thesis (M.A.)--Ohio State University, 2008. / Title from first page of PDF file. Includes bibliographical references (p. 54-62).
120

Class inclusion problem solving, the ability to stop and think, and comparative concept knowledge in young boys.

Landauer, Mari. January 1972 (has links)
Thesis (Ed.D.d)--Teachers College, Columbia University, 1972. / Typescript; issued also on microfilm. Sponsor: Ann E. Boehm. Dissertation Committee: N. Dale Bryant, Janellen Huttenlocher. Includes bibliographical references.

Page generated in 0.1027 seconds