• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 60
  • 6
  • 4
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 98
  • 98
  • 54
  • 38
  • 27
  • 26
  • 19
  • 17
  • 17
  • 15
  • 14
  • 13
  • 13
  • 12
  • 12
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
61

A quantitative study on vocabulary in English textbooks used in upper secondary schools in Sweden

Thi Thuy Dung, Leena Le January 2022 (has links)
Vocabulary plays a fundamental role in language learning acquisition. With a sufficientvocabulary, learners are able to express themselves in various ways, communicate and share their thoughts and opinions with others. In contrast, an inadequate vocabulary gives rise to misunderstanding and poor communication. For many years, one of the most common tools used to enhance and develop learners’ vocabulary in language classrooms have been textbooks. However, to what extent textbooks actually help facilitate and promote vocabulary acquisition has been questioned and debated. Therefore, the present study investigates how English textbooks promote vocabulary acquisition for learners at upper secondary schools in Sweden, and this was done by analysing two English textbooks, namely Blueprint and Solid Gold, used for English 5 in Swedish schools. Firstly, the study examined whether vocabulary in the texts is repeated in the tasks, and if it is, to what extent the vocabulary is repeated. Secondly, the study examined to what extent vocabulary in the textbooks corresponds to CEFR levels targeted in English 5. Lastly, the study examined what types of tasks there are in the textbooks, and whether the textbooks have a focus on explicit or incidental learning. The results showed that there is a lack of repetition in the textbooks. On the other hand, both textbooks corresponded well to CEFR levels targeted in English 5. With respect to different types of tasks, the closed exercise type is the most frequent exercise type in both textbooks. Moreover, there was also a large proportion of communicative tasks in the textbooks. The results also showed that both textbooks seem to have a focus on incidental learning.
62

Second language vocabulary acquisition through storybook reading for Chinese children

Lyu, Ting 01 January 2016 (has links)
This six-week quasi-experimental study compared the effectiveness of two English storybook reading styles (preview-review reading versus concurrent translation) on the vocabulary development of kindergarteners ( n =50) in China, whose primary language is Chinese, who learn English as a second language. The children (aged 5 to 6) were pretested to evaluate their knowledge of general and targeted words using a researcher-designed instrument tailored to the content of three selected English stories. Validity evidence based on contrasted groups and test-retest reliability had been previously gathered for the 24-word instrument (based on 8 words per story) in a pilot study. The children in the concurrent translation group listened to the English storybook with the reader using both English and Chinese interchangeably to assist children in understanding the story. With the children in the preview-review group, the reader built background knowledge and discussed difficult vocabulary in Chinese before reading the storybook only in English then reinforcing key points in Chinese after reading the storybook. Each of three English stories was read three times by the researcher/reader separately to both groups. Two days after the third reading of each story, children were post-tested on the 8 words associated with that specific story. Total posttest scores, based on combining the three 8-word posttest scores, were compared to the 24-word pretest scores to examine vocabulary gains. Results suggest that both methods of reading English storybooks to Chinese preschoolers were effective. Results also indicate that children in the concurrent translation group performed significantly better than children in the preview-review group, even after initial levels of vocabulary, based on the pretest, were controlled (Cohen’s ƒ 2 = .42, which is considered to be a large effect). Additionally, an Aptitude x Treatment Interaction model was tested, but insufficient evidence was found to suggest that the differential effectiveness of the two methods depended on the child’s initial vocabulary level. Implications for teachers, parents, and early childhood education policymakers are discussed and suggestions for further research are offered.
63

Lessons Learned: The Process of Creating and Evaluating an Adult Literacy Curriculum for College Transition

Gravett, Meredith Leigh Packard 10 June 2014 (has links) (PDF)
This thesis documents the process of developing and evaluating a curriculum written for adult literacy students transitioning from their current literacy goals to academic and occupational pursuits. The curriculum, titled the Basic College Skills Transition Curriculum (BCS Transition Curriculum), was written for students at Project Read, a non-profit adult literacy program located at the Provo Library in Provo, Utah. The model used to design this curriculum is a variation on the ADDIE model (analysis, design, development, implementation, and evaluation), including the reiterative steps of research and evaluation. The lesson plans and materials in the BCS Transition Curriculum include the topics of U.S. History Vocabulary, Textbook Reading Skills, Lecture Note-Taking Skills, Study Organization Skills, Test-Taking Skills, Computer Skills, and Time-Management Skills. The curriculum is content-based, with each skill being taught through the content subject of U.S. History. This allows vocabulary to be recycled through each lesson plan and creates more opportunities for vocabulary acquisition. This also ties the curriculum together in an integrated experience that simulates an academic course in U.S. History. After the lesson plans were developed, tutors and students used the lesson plans and participated in an evaluation of the curriculum. Their feedback gave direction for revisions and provided ideas for the future development of this curriculum. Some lessons learned during this project about curriculum design include the importance of 1) including the curriculum designer's needs in the needs assessment; 2) clearly communicating needs and constraints to collaborators and stakeholders; 3) planning for problems and accepting limitations in the product; and 4) collaborating with others.
64

Latinate Word Parts And Vocabulary:contrasts Among Three Groups Comprisingthe Community College Preparatory Reading Class

Bellomo, Tom 01 January 2005 (has links)
Students enrolled in a college preparatory reading class at one particular community college were categorized based on language origin. Native English speaking students comprised one group and foreign students formed two additional groups--students whose language origin was Latin-based (i.e. Romance languages) and students whose language origin was not Latin-based (i.e. Japanese). A pretest assessment measure was used to quantify the extent that pre-existing knowledge of Latinate word parts and morphologically complex vocabulary differed among groups based on language origin. The identical instrument served as a posttest to measure the extent that direct instruction in morphological analysis resulted in change among the same groups after one semester of instruction. Two sections on both the pretest and posttest yielded a total of four distinct mean scores that formed the primary basis for comparison. Categorizing students within the college preparatory reading class based on language origin revealed distinctive strengths and weaknesses relative to group identity when learning Latin-based word parts and vocabulary. Results of a one-way fixed-factor analysis of variance, in conjunction with multiple comparison procedures, indicated that the Latin-based group performed the strongest. This group had the greatest mean score on all four measurements; however, only for the word part section of the pretest was the difference statistically significant. The non Latin-based group performed the poorest as evidenced by scoring the lowest on three of the four measures, with a statistically significant difference for the vocabulary pretest. Additionally, a disproportionately large number of students within the native English-speaking group had difficulty mastering word parts. Though the lower group mean was statistically significant for the word part section of the posttest, practical significance was not observable from the descriptive data. A follow-up frequency tabulation revealed a dichotomization within the native English speaking group between those who proceeded to master word parts and those who did not. Furthermore, results from a pretest/posttest comparison for each respective group indicated that all three groups made significant gains on both sections of the test instrument as a result of direct instruction in Latinate word parts and vocabulary. However, there was an incongruity between word part and vocabulary mastery as all three group means were markedly better on the word part section of the instrument. The results of this study suggest that college preparatory students, regardless of their language origin, enter higher education with limited knowledge of Latinate word parts and vocabulary. The results further suggest that students comprising the heterogeneously populated college preparatory reading class can profit from direct instruction in morphological analysis--regardless of language origin. Prior research has demonstrated that college-level content words tend to be morphologically complex, singular in meaning, and likely to be Latinate in origin. Reading is the salient skill utilized across the curriculum and often the primary means of content dissemination. Reading, in turn, is principally linked to the extent of one's vocabulary. Consequently, teaching morphologically complex vocabulary at the college preparatory level along with providing a working knowledge of morphemes can assist students toward college readiness.
65

The Impact of Gamification on Vocabulary Acquisition : A Comparative Study of Two Student Response Systems in Enhancing Vocabulary Acquisition

Xerri, Cindy January 2024 (has links)
As digitalization continues to advance, digital tools have become omnipresent in the classroom, and Student Response Systems (SRSs) have emerged as a valuable tool for teachers. However, the added value and efficacy of integrating gamified SRSs, compared to non-gamified SRSs, in the classroom has yet to be fully explored. Furthermore, little attention has been paid to non-gamified SRSs. The present study examines how a gamified learning environment in SRSs impacts vocabulary acquisition compared to a non-gamified learning environment. To analyze this effect, a quantitative study was designed using statistical analyses such as the Shapiro-Wilk test, the Friedman test and the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. SRSs, namely Gimkit and Socrative, were selected to measure vocabulary acquisition. A total of 61 high school students studying English were enrolled in this study. Eighteen target words have been picked from two different vocabulary books designed to help students reach the C1 level in English. The selected words have all been identified as adjectives. The students were split into two class-based groups, and each was assigned a SRS to use for two 20-minute study sessions, happening at an interval of three days. To measure the effect of the two SRSs on vocabulary acquisition, three tests were conducted over three weeks. The students took a pre-test, an immediate post-test after the second study session, and a final delayed post-test two weeks later. The statistical analysis and the results of the three tests measuring vocabulary acquisition revealed a statistically significant improvement in vocabulary test scores for both groups in the short- and long-term. However, no statistical significance was shown for the test scores between the two groups: gamified and non-gamified learning environments. Hence, a gamified learning environment using SRSs was shown to have no statistical significance on vocabulary learning and retention compared to a non-gamified one. While digital tools are widely embraced, this study suggests that gamified SRSs, compared to non-gamified SRSs, may not offer a significant advantage in terms of vocabulary acquisition and retention. It is, therefore, important for the teachers to get to know their students and find the type of SRSs that suit their learning style best. Further studies could investigate the impact of gamified and non-gamified SRS on the students’ vocabulary acquisition and motivation over a school year to understand the effects better.
66

Learning biblical hebrew vocabulary : insights from second language vocabulary acquisition

Thompson, Jeremy Paul 12 1900 (has links)
Thesis (DPhil)--Stellenbosch University, 2011. / ENGLISH ABSTRACT: Though Biblical Hebrew (=BH) is no longer a spoken language, students continue to learn it for the purpose of reading, or at least interacting at a deeper level, with the text of the Hebrew Bible. This suggests that BH shares with any modern language learning course the goal of learning to read. One important part of learning to read is the acquisition of an adequate number of vocabulary items. The purpose of this study is to determine which insights from Second Language Vocabulary Acquisition (=SLVA) research and related fields hold the most promise for a new — and possibly more effective — approach to learning BH vocabulary, to evaluate currently existing BH instructional materials in light of these insights, to develop a new approach based on these insights, and to test aspects of the new approach empirically. Researchers in SLVA have uncovered a number of helpful insights concerning how vocabulary and vocabulary learning should be defined as well as concerning how vocabulary is best learned. On the other hand, BH instructional materials reflect little to no influence from these insights. These materials have continued to define vocabulary narrowly as individual words and continued to conceive of vocabulary learning primarily as pairing form and meaning in contrast to the much more sophisticated definitions found in the SLVA literature. For example, SLVA researchers consider items beyond the word level, such as idioms, to be vocabulary (Moon 1997; Lewis 1993, 1997). BH instructional materials have also failed to include a significant number of beneficial Vocabulary Learning Strategies (=VLSs), while including some VLSs that are either intrinsically problematic or problematic in the ways they are employed. For example, the strategy of learning semantically related items together is common in BH instructional materials, though it has been shown to be problematic in a considerable number of experimental studies (e.g. Nation 2000; Finkbeiner & Nicol 2003; Papathanasiou 2009). Since SLVA research has yet to influence BH instructional materials, a new approach to BH vocabulary learning is warranted. This new approach is based on sound theory concerning what vocabulary is and what it means to learn it, while offering learners as many helpful strategies for learning lexical items as possible. To justify this new approach, a set of experimental studies was run including one longitudinal case study and three larger-scale experiments. This testing was partial in nature since it was only possible to test one variable at a time. The testing revealed a number of important areas for future research into BH vocabulary learning. / AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Alhoewel Bybelse Hebreeus (=BH) nie meer gepraat word nie, hou studente aan om dit aan te leer vir die doel om te lees, of ten minste om op ‘n dieper vlak met die oorspronklike teks van die Hebreeuse Bybel om te gaan. Dit impliseer dat die aanleer van BH net soos met die aanleer van moderne tale, ten doel het om die taal te kan lees. Een belangrike aspek van om te leer lees, is om die woordeskat aan te leer wat vir hierdie doel nodig is. Die doel van hierdie studie is om vas te stel watter insigte, verkry uit Tweede Taal Woordeskat-Aanleer (=TTWA) navorsing en ander verwante studievelde, die meeste belofte inhou vir ‘n nuwe – en moontlik meer effektiewe – benadering tot die leer van BH woordeskat; om huidige BH leermateriaal te evalueer, in lig van hierdie insigte; om ‘n nuwe benadering te ontwikkel gebaseer op hierdie insigte; en om aspekte van die nuwe benadering empiries toets. Navorsers in TTWA het al heelwat insig verkry in hoe woordeskat en die aanleer van woordeskat gedefineer behoort te word, sowel as aangaande die mees effektiewe maniere waarop woordeskat aangeleer kan word. Dit lyk egter of beskikbare BH leermanier niks of baie min by hierdie insigte baatgevind het. Hierdie materiaal handhaaf tipies ‘n baie nou definisie van wat woordeskat is, te wete, individuele woorde. Dit beskou dus die aanleer van woordeskat as die aanleer van hoofsaaklik woordpare met verskillende vorme en betekenisse, in plaas daarvan om die meer gesofistikeerde definisies te gebruik wat in TTWA literatuur gevind word. TTWA navorsers beskou, byvoorbeelde idiome, wat uit meer as een woord kan bestaan, ook as woordskat-eenhede (Moon 1997; Lewis 1993, 1997). BH leermateriaal gebruik selde van die groot getal beskikbare nuttige Woordeskat Leer- Strategieë (=WLS). Daar word eerder dikwels strategieë gebruik wat volgens TTWA navorsing nie baie effektief is nie.’n Goeie voorbeeld in hierdie verband is die strategie om semanties-verwante items saam te leer. Empiriese navorsing het aangetoon dat hierdie strategie sonder twyfel problematies kan wees (bv. Nation 2000; Finkbeiner & Nicol 2003; Papathanasiou 2009). Aangesien TTWA feitelik nog geen beduidende invloed op BH leermateriaal gehad het, is ‘n nuwe benadering tot die aanleer van BH woordeskat waarin TTWA insigte verreken word, geregverdig. Hierdie nuwe benadering is gebaseer op ‘n deeglik begrondige teoriese model aangaande wat woordeskat is, en wat dit beteken om dit te leer. Verder kan geput word uit die skat van nuttige strategieë wat al in TTWS geïdentifiseer is om sodoende aan BH leerder strategieë te bied wat hulleself al bewys het. Om die nuwe benadering empiries te begrond, is ‘n reeks eksperimentele studies geloods. Nie alle aspekte van die nuwe model kon sistematies getoets word nie. Die grondslag vir verdere navorsing is egter gelê.
67

Responder por exclusão em crianças com atraso de linguagem / Responding by exclusion in Late Talking Toddlers

Fonseca, Julia 03 June 2016 (has links)
Crianças com atraso de linguagem são geralmente identificadas a partir dos dois anos de idade, e são indicadas por seu vocabulário menor do que de seus pares de mesma idade, ainda que sem nenhuma patologia subjacente. Tarefas de responder por exclusão e dica contextual têm sido empregadas na literatura como meios para estudar as possíveis dificuldades presentes no repertório verbal de crianças com atraso de linguagem. Foram realizados dois estudos voltados para explorar o desempenho desta população nestas tarefas: o primeiro testou o responder por exclusão destas crianças, em tarefas de matching-to-sample para ensino de discriminações condicionais auditivo-visuais com nomes e figuras de objetos. O segundo também utilizou tarefas de matching-to-sample e avaliou o responder por exclusão em sondas com estímulos auditivos com topografia de substantivos e verbos no gerúndio, utilizando como estímulos comparação vídeos de ações e imagens de objetos estáticos. Participaram da pesquisa 64 crianças, sendo 32 com desenvolvimento típico e 32 com atraso de linguagem, com idades entre 3 e 4 anos. Esses estudos tiveram como objetivos gerais: a) investigar a ocorrência de responder por exclusão em relações nome-objeto em crianças com indícios de atraso de linguagem, em comparação com crianças de desenvolvimento típico, e b) verificar a interferência de dicas contextuais no responder por exclusão destas crianças, empregando estímulos com topografia de substantivos e de verbos. Os resultados encontrados em ambos os estudos indicam que crianças com desenvolvimento típico e com atraso de linguagem conseguem apresentar responder por exclusão de maneira consistente uma vez que um procedimento de linha de base garanta a aprendizagem da tarefa. Os resultados do segundo estudo, entretanto, indicam que ambos os grupos apresentaram uma queda brusca em seu resultado em tarefas de responder sob controle da dica lexical. Foram encontradas diferenças durante a aquisição da linha de base de verbos, na qual as crianças com desenvolvimento típico apresentaram um desempenho significativamente melhor do que seus pares com atraso de linguagem. Diferenças significativas entre idades também foram encontradas: entre crianças de 3 anos e 4 anos do mesmo grupo e, entre crianças de 3 anos de grupos de diferentes. O mesmo não foi encontrado em relação aos participantes de 4 anos de grupos diferentes. Estes achados permitem concluir que o atraso de linguagem não interferiu no responder por exclusão, mesmo com estímulos de classes lexicais distintas. Além disso, o treino de linha de base de substantivos e verbos, não foram suficientes para estabelecer efetivamente duas classes de estímulos e manter um responder tão robusto quanto na fase de sondas de exclusão para todas as crianças. / Children with language delay are usually identified by the age of two years old, and are appointed by their smaller vocabulary than peers of the same age, although without any underlying pathology. Tasks of responding by exclusion and contextual cue have been reported in the literature as resources to study the possible difficulties present in the verbal repertoire of children with language delay. Two studies were developed to explore the performance of this population in these tasks: the first tested the responding by exclusion of children in matching-to-sample tasks for the teaching of audio-visual conditional discriminations with names and pictures of objects. The second also used matching-tosample tasks and assessed the responding by exclusion at probes with auditory stimuli with topography of nouns and verbs in gerund, using as stimuli comparison videos of actions and images of static objects. The participants were 64 children, 32 with typical development and 32 with language delay, ages 3 and 4 years-old. These studies had as general objectives: a) to investigate the occurrence of responding by exclusion in objectname relations in children with language delay indications compared with children with typical development, and b) to verify the interference of contextual cues in responding by exclusion, using stimuli with topography of nouns and verbs. The results in both studies indicate that children with normal development and language delay can display responding by exclusion consistently since a baseline procedure ensures learning the task. The results of the second study, however, indicate that both groups exhibited a sudden decrease in their performances in tasks of responding under the control of lexical cue. Differences were found during the acquisition of baseline verbs, in which the typically developed children presented a significantly better performance than children with language delay. Significant differences were also found comparing ages: between children of 3 and 4 years old from the same group, and amid 3 year-old-children from different groups, but the same was not found in relation to the participants of 4 years from different groups. These findings indicate that language delay did not affect the responding by exclusion, even with different lexical classes stimuli. In addition, baseline training with nouns and verbs was not sufficient to establish two classes of effective stimuli and to maintain a responding performance as robust as the exclusion probes phase for all children.
68

Vocabulary Acquisition Based on Nation’s Criteria for Knowing a Word, with a Focus on Proficiency and Frequency : A Study on Incidental Vocabulary Acquisition through Reading and the Role of Surrounding Factors / Vokabulärinlärning utifrån Nations kriterier för att kunna ett ord med fokus på språknivå och ordfrekvens : En studie om vokabulärinlärning som konsekvens av läsning och kringliggande faktorers roll

Erlandsson, Tina, Gutierrez Wallgren, Sara January 2019 (has links)
Several studies have been made in the field of second language acquisition (SLA) regarding incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading. However, the majority have focused on the meaning of a word to measure complete acquisition. Nation (2001) argues that there are three main criteria for knowing a word, namely form, meaning and use, and it is not until all three criteria are met that one acquires new vocabulary. Therefore, we chose to create a study which focuses on incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading, but that focuses on three sub-criteria of Nation’s three main ones, namely recognition, association and collocation. In a previous study (Erlandsson and G. Wallgren 2017) we concluded that higher vocabulary knowledge contributes to better reading comprehension. Additionally, researchers (Horst et al. 1998; Day et al. 1991; Zahar et al. 2001; Waring and Takaki 2003; Pigada and Smith 2006, and Zhao et al. 2016) have also brought up several factors, such as learners’ prior proficiency level and word frequency, that can affect the outcome of incidental vocabulary acquisition. Therefore, we decided to investigate what impact these two factors have as well.  Our research questions are: How much vocabulary is learnt incidentally through reading, and how do proficiency and word frequency affect incidental vocabulary acquisition? These questions were answered through a study made in a classroom environment with students in the 8th grade. We were inspired by a study made by Waring and Takaki (2003) who focused on two main criteria for knowing a word, form and meaning. Our study was done through reading nine chapters from the novel Holes by Louis Sachar (2001) and to determine the degree to which rate word frequency played a part in incidental vocabulary acquisition, 24 words were chosen within four different ranges of word frequency (ranging between two occurrences to 39 occurrences in the text). These 24 words were then replaced with substitute words to ensure that each test word was new to the participants. First, the participants completed a reading comprehension test to establish the participants’ reading proficiency levels in English. They were later asked to read the chapters containing the substitute words. Directly after the reading exercise, the participants completed a vocabulary acquisition test. The vocabulary acquisition test consisted of three parts that focused on recognition (word recognition), association (multiple choice) and collocation (putting the target words in a context). Results show that words are acquired incidentally through reading. Our findings show a positive correlation between high reading proficiency levels and a higher amount of words acquired. The findings also indicate a positive correlation between words within a higher frequency range with a higher chance of being acquired. Furthermore, we also observed that substitute words with low frequency in some situations had a higher uptake than those words with a higher frequency. After this observation we tried to explain the anomaly by looking into the textual context of the surrounding words and found a potential explanation in the fact that the low frequency words had very descriptive surroundings.
69

Twist in the list : frame semantics as vocabulary teaching and learning tool

Atzler, Judith Kerstin 31 May 2011 (has links)
The purpose of this study, which is grounded in applied linguistics, was to investigate two ways of presenting vocabulary in a German language class in order to determine whether Frame Semantics is a feasible tool with regards to students’ vocabulary acquisition and culturally appropriate usage of vocabulary. In addition, this study examined learners’ attitudes toward the new method of vocabulary teaching and learning. A total of 34 university students enrolled in four second-semester German classes participated in this study. In the Control Group rote memorization techniques were used, while the in the Treatment Group frame semantics was utilized for the teaching and learning of vocabulary. The data was analyzed through quantitative methods. The quantitative data was derived from an online demographic survey, a vocabulary pre-test, two vocabulary post-tests (an immediate post-test and delayed post-test), a cultural appropriateness pre-test, two cultural appropriateness post-tests (an immediate post-test and delayed post-test), as well as an pre-test and post-test attitude scale provided as an online questionnaire. Analysis of the data indicates that there was no statistically significant difference between the two groups with regards to their cultural appropriate usage of the vocabulary items, and no statistically significant differences were observed with regards to vocabulary recall and retention. In addition, only the factor of enjoyment yielded significant differences with regards to learners’ attitude, while the factors of motivation, interest and confidence did not show statistically significant differences between the groups. Thus, the results indicate that both methods – Frame Semantics and the more traditional methods – are suitable for vocabulary learning and teaching as both methods resulted in an increase of learners’ vocabulary knowledge, including long-term retention. / text
70

網絡圖教學對高中生英文字彙習得之效益研究:以The Frayer Model為基礎 / The effects of graphic organizer instruction on English vocabulary acquisition of senior high school students: Take the Frayer Model as the basis

吳依婷, Wu, Yi Ting Unknown Date (has links)
本研究旨在探討網絡圖教學對高中生英文字彙習得的影響。網絡圖已廣泛運用於閱讀教學,且the Frayer model之前的研究都專注在學科領域的單字與閱讀教學。然而在以英語為第二外語的環境中,針對使用the Frayer model於網絡圖教學是否能增進學習者字彙習得(vocabulary acquisition)與記憶保留(word retention),其相關研究仍顯不足。因此,本研究採用量化研究方法,以探究「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」對高中生英文字彙習得與記憶保留的影響。 研究對象為台灣北部一所公立高中一年級兩個班的六十九位學生,具有相同英文能力的這兩個班級被隨機指定為實驗組與控制組。在歷時六週的實驗教學中,實驗組與控制組分別接受「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」來學習三十二個主要單字。之後,兩組受試者隨即接受包含所有主要單字的研究者自編測驗,以得知兩組受試者的單字習得情形。一個月後,兩組受試者再次接受相同的單字測驗,以進一步得知單字的記憶保留情形。本研究主要發現如下:(1)整體而言,接受「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」的實驗組學生習得大多數的主要單字,但單字保留成效不佳;(2)總括來說,接受「純傳統單字教法」的控制組學生習得並保留大多數的主要單字;(3)在單字習得與記憶保留成效方面,接受「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」的實驗組學生顯著優於接受「純傳統單字教法」的控制組學生;(4)「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」對於高英語學習成就者的單字習得與記憶保留皆有正面影響,但只對低英語學習成就者的單字習得有正面影響;(5)對於高英語學習成就者之單字習得而言,「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」的成效優於「純傳統單字教法」:然而,對於單字記憶保留而言,「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」的成效相同。對於低英語學習成就者之單字習得與記憶保留而言,「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」的成效相同。最後,研究者針對「網絡圖暨傳統單字教法」與「純傳統單字教法」在實際教學上的應用提供建議,以作為教育學者們的參考。 / This study intends to explore the effects of graphic organizer instruction on vocabulary acquisition of senior high school students. Though graphic organizers have been widely employed in reading instruction and previous studies on the Frayer model focus on vocabulary and reading in content areas, little research has been conducted on whether graphic organizer instruction utilizing the adapted Frayer model will facilitate learners’ vocabulary acquisition and word retention in an EFL context. Therefore, this present study adopted a quantitative research method to investigate the effectiveness of two different vocabulary instructions, i.e., “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” and “traditional vocabulary instruction only” on senior high school students’ vocabulary acquisition and word retention. Participants of this study were two classes of 69 first year students in a public senior high school in northern Taiwan. With similar English proficiency on General English Proficiency Test (GEPT), the two classes were randomly assigned as the experimental and control groups. During the six-week instructional experiment, the experimental and control groups received “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” and “traditional vocabulary instruction only” respectively to learn 32 target words selected in this study. After receiving the last vocabulary instructions, both groups took the immediate post-test, a researcher self-designed test, to assess their acquisition of all the target words. One month after the immediate post-test, both groups received the delayed post-test, which was the same as the immediate post-test, to track their word retention. The major findings are summarized as follows. (1) As a whole, learners receiving “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” acquired most of the target words but did not retain them one month after. (2) Overall, learners receiving “traditional vocabulary instruction only” not only acquired the target words but also retained them in a month. (3) In terms of vocabulary acquisition and word retention, learners receiving “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” significantly performed better than those receiving “traditional vocabulary instruction only”. (4) Both “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” and “traditional vocabulary instruction only” promoted high proficiency learners’ vocabulary acquisition and word retention, but were only effective in low proficiency learners’ vocabulary acquisition. (5) High proficiency learners receiving “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” significantly acquired much more target words than those receiving “traditional vocabulary instruction only”, but similarly retained target words as those receiving “traditional vocabulary instruction only”. Low proficiency learners receiving “graphic organizer plus traditional vocabulary instruction” and “traditional vocabulary instruction only” acquired target words as well as retained the words to a similar extent. Pedagogical implications and recommendations for further research were provided at the end of the thesis.

Page generated in 0.0368 seconds