Spelling suggestions: "subject:"nonverbal behavior"" "subject:"converbal behavior""
131 |
Seleção cultural e transmissão cultural no jogo Dilema do prisioneiro iterado / Cultural selection and cultural transmission in game Iterated prisoner’s dilemmaPin, Stephanny Sato Del 26 May 2017 (has links)
Submitted by Filipe dos Santos (fsantos@pucsp.br) on 2017-06-01T13:00:08Z
No. of bitstreams: 1
Stephanny Sato Del Pin.pdf: 1561148 bytes, checksum: 1301b79efce9f4d94d7086a9c79e84bc (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2017-06-01T13:00:08Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1
Stephanny Sato Del Pin.pdf: 1561148 bytes, checksum: 1301b79efce9f4d94d7086a9c79e84bc (MD5)
Previous issue date: 2017-05-26 / The cooperation experiments discuss important parameters about the social behavior and the social environment, using the triple contingencies unit of analysis. The iterated prisoners dilemma game was repeatedly used as a procedure for experimental investigation into cooperation by behavioral analysts. The prisoners dilemma consists of the conflicting relation between individual gains and group gains. The combination of the choices among all those involved in the dilemma may lead to: losses for all, gains for all or loss for one while another gains. Such analyses of cooperation may be expanded when another unit of analysis – metacontingency – is used. This study sought replicate the work of Ortu et al. (2012), investigating the effect of cultural consequences on the selection of interlocking behavioral contingencies. Three modifications were made: 1) open groups were programmed – replacement of participants 2) the presentation of cultural consequences (market feedback) was made on a CRF schedule; and to contribute to the comprehension of verbal behavior in the selection by metacontingencies 3) the rules issued by the participants throughout the generations were analysed. Each of the three experimental groups, while in the experimental context, built a “cultural practice” as a form of coordination to obtain individual points and/or points of cultural consequences forecast in the various experimental conditions. Exposure to all experimentally programmed cultural consequences was possible for all groups. Groups 1 and 3 varied in the production of interlacements in initial generations, but on the whole met the required performance criteria, therefore they were consistent in the production of cooperational interlacements. Group 2 produced variable interlacements, the most consistent being XYYY and YYYX, i.e. “competitive” interlacements. The analysis of verbal interactions clarifies the reason behind such production: to obtain more points individually, group 2 coordinated themselves on a rotating schedule, whereby whoever chooses differently to the others would have a greater gain, while the others would have a medium gain, and would not receive any additional gain in the cultural consequence. The participants in group 2 obtained the total egalitarian gains and coordinated themselves with greater frequency from informative rules. Group 3 coordinated themselves with greater frequency from coersive rules and had unequal individual totals. The results of this study suggest that the selection of cooperation standards depends on the individuals consequences forecast in the more “traditional” procedures, but also depends significantly on external variables studied here: the production of cultural consequences contingent on aggregated products and the analysis of verbal interaction for the selection by metacontingencies / Os experimentos de cooperação discutem importantes parâmetros sobre o comportamento social e o ambiente social, utilizando a unidade de análise tríplice contingências. O jogo dilema do prisioneiro iterado foi recorrentemente utilizado como um procedimento para investigação experimental sobre cooperação por analistas do comportamento. O dilema do prisioneiro consiste na relação de conflito entre ganhos individuais e ganhos para o grupo. A combinação das escolhas dentre todos os envolvidos no dilema pode acarretar em: prejuízos para todos, ganhos para todos, ou prejuízo para um, enquanto há ganho para o outro. Tais análises sob cooperação podem ser ampliadas quando utiliza-se outra unidade de análise, a metacontingência. Este estudo buscou replicar o trabalho de Ortu et al. (2012), investigando o efeito das consequências culturais para a seleção de contingências comportamentais entrelaçadas. Três modificações foram realizadas: 1) foram programados grupos abertos - substituição de participantes 2) a apresentação de consequências culturais (feedback de mercado) foi realizada em um esquema de CRF; e para contribuir para a compreensão do comportamento verbal na seleção por metacontingências 3) as regras emitidas pelos participantes ao longo das gerações foram analisadas. Cada um dos três grupos experimentais, ainda que no contexto experimental, construiu uma “prática cultural” como forma de coordenação para obtenção dos pontos individuais e/ou das consequências culturais previstas nas várias condições experimentais. Para todos os grupos foi possível a exposição à todas consequências culturais programadas experimentalmente. Os grupos 1 e 3 ao início durante as gerações iniciais variaram na produção dos entrelaçamentos, mas, em sua maioria, atingiram o critério de desempenho exigido, portanto, foram consistentes na produção de entrelaçamentos de cooperação. O grupo 2 produziu variavelmente os entrelaçamentos, sendo o mais consistente XYYY e YYYX, entrelaçamentos ditos como “competitivos”. A análise das interações verbais esclarece o porquê de tal produção, o grupo 2, para que obtivessem mais pontos individualmente coordenou-se em um arranjo de “rodízio”, aquele que escolhesse diferente dos demais teria o ganho mais vantajoso, enquanto que os demais o ganho médio, e não receberiam nenhum ganho adicional na consequência cultural. Os participantes do grupo 2 obtiveram o total de ganhos individuais aproximadamente iguais e coordenaram-se com maior frequência a partir de regras informativas. O grupo 3 coordenou-se mais frequentemente a partir de regras coercitivas e obtiveram totais individuais desiguais. Os resultados do presente estudo indicam que a seleção de padrões de cooperação depende das consequências individuais previstas nos procedimentos mais “tradicionais”, mas depende também e de maneira importante de variáveis externas aqui investigadas: a produção de consequências culturais contingentes a produtos agregados, e a interação verbal que favorece a coordenação para a seleção por metacontingências
|
132 |
Vocal and Non-Vocal Verbal Behavior Between Mothers and Their Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum DisorderGreer, Ashley Briggs January 2018 (has links)
I conducted a descriptive analysis on the emission of vocal and non-vocal social/verbal interactions between 35 dyads of preschool-aged-children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and their mothers. Using previously recorded videos of 5-min isolated free-play sessions between the mother-child dyads, I transduced each occurrence of verbal operants, attempted verbal operants (i.e., emissions not reinforced by a listener), and additional verbal behaviors such as fantasy play emitted by the child, and approvals and disapprovals emitted by the mother. Each verbal behavior was defined as either vocal verbal behavior, non-lexical vocal verbal behavior, or non-vocal verbal behavior, all with a function to communicate. The procedure consisted of identifying each instance of verbal behavior emitted between the mother and child rotating across listener and speaker responses until either no response occurred, or the session concluded. The listener and speaker responses were further transduced into individual initiated conversational units (speaker-listener-speaker rotations). These data were statistically analyzed with previously collected child educational variables and mother demographic variables: child's level of verbal behavior in accordance with the Verbal Behavior Developmental Assessment-Revised (VBDA-R), number of acquired objectives on the Comprehensive Application of Behavior Analysis to Schooling International Curriculum and Inventory of Repertoires for Children from Preschool through Kindergarten (C-PIRK), the Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2) severity scores, and the scores on the Vineland-3 Adaptive Behavior Scales-Third Edition (VABS-3). The mother demographic variables were level of education and household income. The results of the study were as follow: (1) a significant relationship was shown between the child's level of verbal behavior (extracted from the VBDA-R) and performance on the C-PIRK, VABS-3, and between the ADOS-2 Modules used to assess for ASD severity; (2) the results did not show a significant difference between the child's level of verbal behavior and the number of child-initiated conversational units. The differences in the verbal behavior exchanged between the mother and child were, however, indicated across the child's form of verbal behavior - vocal, non-lexical, and non-vocal verbal behavior - emitted with the mother. Results are interpreted as parents of children without vocal verbal behavior require parent training tailored to their child's verbal developmental repertoires rather than their chronological age to ensure all communicative opportunities are captured. Educational implications, limitations, and future avenues of research are discussed.
|
133 |
The Effects of Naming Experiences and Properties of Visual Stimuli on Language Acquisition and the Relationship between Curiosity and NamingOrlans, Sarah Elizabeth January 2017 (has links)
Children typically acquire language rapidly during their first few years of life. Their rates and levels of proficiency vary, but it is clear that the development of one’s language repertoire impacts academic outcomes and future success across many domains. There are both genetic and environmental factors that affect and contribute to one’s development. For children whose vocal verbal behavior is less well developed, it is imperative that we continue to develop and implement tactics and procedures to intervene in order to accelerate their language development. Researchers have identified Naming as a critical verbal developmental capability that allows one to learn language incidentally. Are there different types of Naming capabilities? Do properties of stimuli affect language acquisition? Does the Naming repertoire relate to children’s level of curiosity about the world around them? In the 3 experiments that follow, I examined the effects of 2 types of Naming experiences and varying properties of visual stimuli on measures of Naming. In Experiments 2 and 3, I also conducted measures of curiosity to assess the possibility of a relationship between Naming and question asking. In my first experiment there were 31 participants. I investigated the effects of match-to-sample and exclusion Naming experiences on incidental acquisition of listener and speaker responses in both adults without disabilities and youth with disabilities. I examined the differences between the 2 age groups and Naming experiences. The adult means of listener and speaker responses were greater than the youth means. All adults met criterion for Naming with the match-to-sample experience, and 9 of 14 adults also achieved criterion levels with the unfamiliar stimuli following the exclusion Naming experience. The adult group’s results showed that the group’s Naming repertoire was fairly balanced for listener responses across the Naming experiences with minimal variability, and its speaker repertoire was not as balanced. The youth group’s results demonstrated similar levels of variability across both topographies. The effect of the Naming experience was significant for speaker responses. In the second experiment, I implemented an intervention to try to establish unfamiliar stimuli as reinforcers to test its effects on the 2 types of Naming probes and curiosity measures in 6 elementary age children with disabilities. There were some effects from the treatment, but following 2 intervention conditions none of the participants met criteria for Naming. The participants’ numbers of accurate listener responses were greater than their speaker responses. In Experiment 3, I conducted tests for curiosity and Naming with sets of stimuli that had varying levels of familiarity and complexity for 9 preschool age children with and without disabilities. As with the first 2 experiments, the numbers of listener responses for participants were greater than their speaker responses, and there was more variability in the speaker responses compared to the listener responses. The results suggested that the type of Naming experience or the familiarity level of the visual stimuli alone did not appear to influence the dependent variables, but rather that there may be an interaction among the independent variables. The means of responses were greater with more familiar stimuli following match-to-sample experiences whereas the means were greater with less familiar stimuli following the exclusionary Naming experiences. The results of the 3 experiments affirmed the independence of the listener and speaker components of Naming and suggest that the demonstration of Naming with unknown, unfamiliar types of stimuli may be a type of Naming capability that may not be present in all individuals who demonstrate Naming with unknown, familiar stimuli.
|
134 |
CONDIÇÕES DE EXPERIMENTAÇÃO INFERENCIAL PARA INVESTIGAÇÃO DOS EVENTOS PRIVADOSSilva, André Vasconcelos da 31 August 2001 (has links)
Submitted by admin tede (tede@pucgoias.edu.br) on 2017-12-13T18:25:08Z
No. of bitstreams: 1
André Vasconcelos da Silva.pdf: 407291 bytes, checksum: c022b4064ab7bf7e5390618234452130 (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2017-12-13T18:25:08Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1
André Vasconcelos da Silva.pdf: 407291 bytes, checksum: c022b4064ab7bf7e5390618234452130 (MD5)
Previous issue date: 2001-08-31 / Skinner outlined the conditions to inquire into private events in 1945. Two experiments
were conducted to determine the efficacy of experimental procedures, which set up
correlations between private events and verbal responses, and to dispose of usage
conditions of the term “private event”. The aim of the Experiment 1 was to investigate
the mutual relations between verbal reports indicating private events and public
behaviors (description of contingencies and problem solving). Eight undergraduate
students without any previous experimental history were exposed to three experimental
conditions: Baseline, Training and Extinction. The contingencies description reports and
problem solving responses were recorded in each experimental condition. Nevertheless,
in the Training condition, the responses indicating privacy (informative responses) were
registered as well. Correlation between informative responses and description responses
was established. In spite of it, any correlation involving problem solving responses
could not be observed. The purpose of the Experiment 2 was to verify the effect of
previous reinforcement contingencies upon the inter-relation between public behaviors
and informative responses. Five undergraduate students without any previous
experimental history of instrumental conditioning were exposed to two experimental
sessions. In the Session 1, correlation between contingencies description responses and
problem solving responses was established. The Session 2 was similar to Experiment 1,
but the contingencies arrangement was not the same used in the Session 1. Data analysis
showed correlation between informative responses and description and problem solving
responses in the Session 1, when the participants responses were not under contingency
control of the Session 2. Moreover, when the responses were under contingencies
control of the Session 2, they were related to them. The experiments 1 and 2 provide
empirical evidencies of the correlation between observable events and private events.
The results of both experiments also give consent for the establishment of experimental
usage conditions of the term “private event” by means of controlling variable detection. / Skinner em 1945 estabeleceu condições para se investigar os eventos privados. Na
tentativa de se verificar a eficácia de procedimentos e experimentações que estabelecem
correlações dos eventos privados com respostas verbais e dispor condições de uso do
termo evento privado, realizou-se dois experimentos. O Experimento 1, objetivou
verificar a correlação dos relatos verbais públicos indicativos dos eventos privados,
Resposta de Informação, com comportamentos públicos: Descrição das Contingências e
Resolução do Problema. Participaram do experimento oito alunos universitários sem
história experimental que foram submetidos a três fases experimentais: Linha de base,
Treino e Extinção. Na fase de Treino os participantes foram distribuídos em duas
condições que disponibilizavam diferentes possibilidades de descrição das
contingências: a) condição Grupo Relato a Cada Sim e b) condição Grupo Relato ao
Final. Nas fases Linha de base e Extinção registrou-se as respostas de descrição das
contingências e de resolução do problema e, na fase Treino, adicionou-se o registro das
respostas indicativas dos eventos privados. Observou-se que as Respostas de
Informação correlacionavam-se às Respostas de Descrição, porém não foi possível
detectar correlação com as Respostas de Resolução. Não foi possível inferir neste
experimento quais os eventos privados participaram da cadeia comportamental, devido
a impossibilidade de se verificar correlações. O Experimento 2 objetivou verificar o
efeito de contingências prévias de reforçamento sobre a correlação existente entre as
Respostas de Informação e os comportamentos de descrição e resolução. Participaram
cinco alunos universitários sem experiência prévia em experimentos de
condicionamento operante. Os participantes foram expostos a duas sessões: a Sessão 1,
que estabeleceu correlação entre Respostas de Descrição da contingência e as Respostas
de Resolução; e a Sessão 2, semelhante ao Experimento 1, incluindo na distribuição dos
participantes nas condições de relato. A Sessão 1 diferiu da Sessão 2 basicamente nas
contingências, em que na Sessão 1 os estímulos discriminativos utilizados foram letras e
na Sessão 2 foram palavras. Os resultados permitem observar que as respostas de
informações correlacionaram-se à descrição e resolução da Sessão 1, quando os
participantes não estavam sob o controle da contingência da Sessão 2, e posteriormente
passaram a se correlacionar às contingências da Sessão 2, quando ficaram sob controle
destas mesmas contingências. Isso para condição do Grupo Relato a Cada Sim. Para a
condição Relato ao Final se inferiu acerca da relação de comparação estabelecida entre
os desempenhos de Descrição e Resolução dos participantes de ambas as condições.
Com isso, os experimentos possibilitaram inferir sob quais condições podem-se detectar
correlação entre eventos observáveis e os eventos privados.
|
135 |
ESTUDOS DE VARIÁVEIS DE CONTEXTO EM UM EPISÓDIO VERBALGomes, Ueliton dos Santos 31 March 2017 (has links)
Submitted by admin tede (tede@pucgoias.edu.br) on 2018-02-07T18:14:43Z
No. of bitstreams: 1
UELITON DOS SANTOS GOMES.pdf: 681227 bytes, checksum: aa39200339444f538faecb90562fa738 (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2018-02-07T18:14:43Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1
UELITON DOS SANTOS GOMES.pdf: 681227 bytes, checksum: aa39200339444f538faecb90562fa738 (MD5)
Previous issue date: 2017-03-31 / The goal of the present work was: to investigate the relations between the independent
variables, which in this study were verbal (instructions) and non-verbal (figures and
images of objects) contexts and their effects in the dependent variable, which was the
descriptive behavior of OBJECTS and ACTIONS in a total verbal episode. We had also a
secondary goal: the development of a software for the collect and analysis of data. An
experiment was conducted as a systematic replication of the work by Simonassi, Tizo,
Gomes e Alvarenga (2010) in order to verify: 1) if verbal and non-verbal contexts exert
control over verbal responses in a total verbal episode. This experiment was conducted
with ten (10) participants with ages varying from 19 to 25 yeas. Each participant was
exposed to 4 different experimental conditions (CONDITIONS 1, 2, 3 and 4) in a singlecase
design. During the CONDITIONS 1, 2 and 3 the following instructions was
presented: “BURNING THIS TIP HAS BEEN”, being that in the CONDITIONS 2 and 3
the following non-verbal stimuli were presented: MATCHES and a STRING (figures and
images) and in the CONDITION 3: MATCHES and a CANDLE (figures and images). In
the CONDITION 4 the instruction presented was “BURN IT ON THIS TIP. CALM SIR,
STILL ENDING STRUCTURING CLOTHES”. In each condition the participant was
asked to write his answers. A categorization of the textual responses as referent to
OBJECT or ACTION was used for analysis. We verified a considerable frequency of
textual verbal responses that made reference to the related objects (figures/images) and
actions. The results were similar to those found by Simonassi & Cols. We concluded that
both the verbal (instruction) and non-verbal (objects) contexts, when altered, controlled
changes in the participants’ responses across the experimental conditions in a systematic
way. / O presente trabalho teve como objetivo investigar as relações entre as variáveis
independentes, que neste estudo foram os contextos verbais (instrução) e não-verbais
(Figura/imagem de objeto) e seu efeito na variável dependente, que foram os
comportamentos que descreviam respostas referentes à OBJETOS e a AÇÃO em um
episódio verbal total. Teve ainda como objetivo secundário o desenvolvimento de um
software para a realização da coleta e análise de dados. Foi realizado um experimento de
replicação sistemática do procedimento adotado no estudo de Simonassi e colaboradores
para verificar: 1) se contextos verbais (instruções) e não verbais (objetos) exercem controle
sobre respostas verbais em um episódio verbal total. Este experimento foi realizado com
dez (10) participantes com idades entre 19 a 25 anos. Cada participante foi submetido a 4
condições experimentais diferentes (CONDIÇÃO 1, 2, 3 e 4) em um delineamento de
sujeito como seu próprio controle. Nas CONDIÇÕES 1, 2 e 3 foi apresentada a seguinte
instrução “QUEIME LOGO ESTA PONTA AÍ”, sendo que nas CONDIÇÕES 2 e 3 foram
apresentados os seguintes estímulos não-verbais CONDIÇÃO 2: FÓSFORO e
BARBANTE (Figuras/imagens) e na CONDIÇÃO 3: FÓSFORO e VELA
(Figuras/imagens). E na CONDIÇÃO 4 foi apresentado a instrução ““QUEIME LOGO
ESTA PONTA AÍ. CALMA SENHOR, SENÃO ACABO ESTRAGANDO A ROUPA”.
Em cada condição o participante foi solicitado a responder suas respostas por escrito. A
análise foi realizada com a categorização das respostas textuais referentes a OBJETO e a
AÇÃO. Verificou-se que uma frequência considerável de respostas verbais textuais que
faziam referencias aos objetos relacionados (Figuras/imagens) a ações. Os resultados
foram semelhantes ao do Experimento de Simonassi e Cols. Concluiu-se que tanto o
contexto verbal (instrução) quanto o contexto não-verbal (objetos) quando alterados,
também alteravam sistematicamente o responder dos participantes nas diversas condições
experimentais.
|
136 |
Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essential for Living Communication Modality AssessmentOrozco, Daniella 26 July 2018 (has links)
McGreevy, Fry, and Cornwall (2014) developed an assessment within the Essential for Living (EFL) manual for clinicians to identify which communication modality should be used for each individual. This assessment identifies an AAC based on the learner’s skills, level of problem behavior, similarities between AAC and vocal community, and size of the verbal community. However, to date, no research has evaluated if this assessment identifies the communication modality that will result in faster acquisition of mands in individuals with ASD. Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare acquisition of mands across a modality identified by the EFL communication modality assessment and two other commonly used modalities. A secondary purpose was to determine if participants acquire mands using the mode of AAC identified by EFL. Finally, a third purpose was to determine if the communication modality identified by EFL communication modality assessment matches the modality currently used by the individual. Findings showed that although all three participants acquired mands across the three communication modalities, mands in the modality of communication recommended by the EFL assessment were acquired faster only by 1 out of the 3 participants.
|
137 |
Assessing the Effects of Derived Relational Responding on Intraverbal Use of Same-Opposite and More Than-Less Than Relations in Children with AutismWhite, Jane P. 30 October 2014 (has links)
Relational Frame Theory provides an analysis of verbal behavior involving a focus on the development of relational operants which are seen as a basis for language. From this basis, a framework is provided for establishing relational networks in individuals who lack derived relational ability. Establishment of relational frames may increase the probability of responding relationally to novel instances and use of the specific relational frames during social interactions; therefore, training verbal relations in accordance with an RFT approach may enhance intraverbal responding and facilitate the emergence of untrained responses. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the emergence of specific relationships in the context of intraverbal responding as a collateral effect of training on relational networks in four children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Two participants demonstrated mastery of derived relational responding (DRR) without training, one participant demonstrated mastery of DRR following training, and a fourth participant demonstrated mutual entailment and some combinatorial entailment. Increases in vocal verbal behavior during generalization probes were observed, although increased use of all target relations was not observed in all participants. Further research is needed to evaluate specific deficits in derived relational responding among individuals with ASD, as well as the correlation between DRR and language ability.
|
138 |
Impact of Self-Monitoring and Video Feedback on Staff Implementation of Natural Environment Teaching for Children with ASDTenowich, Heatherann 07 July 2014 (has links)
Children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) characteristically exhibit social communication and language deficits. Natural environment teaching (NET) is an intervention proven effective at improving social communication and language skills. Treatment fidelity is critical for an intervention to be effective. Research shows that training alone is often not sufficient to ensure proper implementation. Self-monitoring has shown to increase treatment fidelity, but performance feedback may be necessary to further improve the fidelity of individuals implementing interventions. Using a nonconcurrent multiple baseline design across participants with an ABC sequence, this study evaluated the impact of self-monitoring and video feedback on behavior therapist implementation of NET procedures and maintenance of the skills during fading. Results indicated that self-monitoring slightly improved fidelity of implementation. Fidelity improved further with the addition of video feedback. The results also indicated that implementation of NET with fidelity by the therapists led to improvement in participating children's verbal language skills.
|
139 |
Relationship of pauses to problem solving events in mechanical design protocolsChamberlin, Martha J. 15 May 1990 (has links)
This thesis compares two methods for studying the problem-solving processes of
mechanical design engineers. The first method, verbal protocol analysis, was applied
by L. Stauffer to construct a problem-solving model of mechanical design. The
second method, timing analysis, measures the time intervals separating drawing or
speaking actions during the design process. Timing analyisis was applied by the
author to the verbal/video design data collected by Stauffer. This thesis demonstrates
that the two methods are statistically related, and hence, that employing
two different study techniques enhances the reliability of both methods. The two
methods have complementary strengths: protocol analysis reveals the content of the
design process, while timing analysis is much more complete. Hence, a combination
of protocol and timing analysis provides a stronger measure of the design process
than either method alone. / Graduation date: 1992
|
140 |
The Effects of Manipulating Conditioned Establishing Operations on the Acquisition of Mands in Children with Autism Spectrum DisordersTroconis, Claudia 01 January 2011 (has links)
In Verbal Behavior, Skinner (1957) suggested that each verbal operant has independent response functions, in which acquiring one does not automatically result in the other, unless transfer between the verbal operants is directly trained. Although several researchers have shown that mands and tacts are functionally independent, more recent research has demonstrated that mands may emerge following tact training. However, this research has not clarified the influence of establishing operations on the emergence of pure mands following tact training. Therefore, the present study investigated the effects of tact training on the acquisition of impure and pure mands in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) when conditioned establishing operations (CEO) were manipulated during mand probes. Three children diagnosed with ASD were taught to tact the utensils needed to consume their preferred edibles and then were assessed on their ability to mand for those utensils during CEO absent versus CEO present pure mand probes using a multiple baseline design across participants. It was hypothesized that children would be able to mand for the missing utensils needed to consume their preferred edibles only when the food items were present (CEO present, pure mand probes), but not when they were absent (CEO absent, pure mand probes). Results showed that responses taught as tacts failed to transfer to mand responses until direct training was implemented for two of the three participants. However, once a mand response was learned, all participants exhibited the mand in the CEO present condition but not in the CEO absent condition.
|
Page generated in 0.0478 seconds