• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 18
  • 11
  • 4
  • Tagged with
  • 33
  • 29
  • 29
  • 24
  • 17
  • 15
  • 15
  • 15
  • 15
  • 12
  • 12
  • 11
  • 11
  • 8
  • 7
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
1

Behavioraler und hirnphysiologischer Zusammenhang von Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung bei adulter Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung / Behavioural and brain physiological association of working memory and response inhibition in adult Attention Deficit-/Hyperactivity Disorder

Schecklmann, Martin January 2009 (has links) (PDF)
Hintergrund: Die Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) umfasst die klinischen Symptome Hyperaktivität, Impulsivität und Unaufmerksamkeit und besitzt eine große erbliche Komponente. Veränderungen des Dopaminstoffwechsels und des präfrontalen Kortex scheinen mit der Erkrankung assoziiert zu sein. Bekannt sind auch Defizite in exekutiven Funktionen wie Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung, welche gemeinsam als ein Endophänotyp der ADHS betrachtet werden. Solche sogenannten intermediären Phänotypen bilden möglicherweise ätiopathogenetisch sinnvollere Untergruppen von Krankheitsbildern als die Unterteilung nach den klassischen Diagnosesystemen. Zahlreiche Untersuchungen zu Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung bei ADHS finden Mittelwertsunterschiede im Vergleich zu gesunden Stichproben in behavioralen und hirnphysiologischen Maßen. Ein genetischer Polymorphismus (Val158Met) mit Einfluss auf die Synthese der Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) moduliert Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung bei Gesunden und Patienten mit ADHS. COMT ist ein Enzym, das den Abbau von präfrontalem Dopamin katalysiert. Das Valin(Val)-Allel ist mit einer geringeren, das Methionin(Met)-Allel mit einer höheren Verfügbarkeit von Dopamin in kortikalen Arealen assoziiert. Letzteres scheint die Funktion präfrontaler Hirnareale zu optimieren. Ziel: Es ist unklar, ob die beiden Prozesse Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung miteinander in Verbindung stehen, also einen gemeinsamen Endophänotypen bezeichnen, und ob ein möglicher Zusammenhang dieser beiden Funktionen durch Dopamin moduliert wird. Gegenstand der vorliegenden Arbeit ist, (1) die bekannten Mittelwertsunterschiede zwischen Patienten mit ADHS und gesunden Kontrollpersonen zu replizieren, (2) den Zusammenhang von Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung mit Korrelationsanalysen zu überprüfen, (3) den Einfluss des COMT-Polymorphismus auf Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung zu untersuchen und (4) festzustellen, ob der COMT-Polymorphismus die Stärke der Korrelationen beeinflusst. Die Fragestellungen 3 und 4 sind aufgrund der kleinen Stichproben als explorativ zu bewerten. Methoden: In die Auswertung aufgenommen wurden 45 erwachsene Patienten mit ADHS und 41 nach Alter, Geschlecht, Händigkeit, Intelligenz, Bildungsniveau und Kopfumfang vergleichbare Kontrollen. Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung wurden durch eine N-Back- bzw. eine Stoppsignal-Aufgabe operationalisiert. Als abhängige Variablen dienten als behaviorale Maße die Anzahl richtiger Antworten, Reaktionszeiten und Effizienz und als hirnphysiologische Parameter Konzentrationsveränderungen von oxygeniertem (O2Hb) und deoxygeniertem Hämoglobin (HHb) in präfrontalen Arealen, die mittels funktioneller Nah-Infrarot Spektroskopie gemessen wurden. Korrelationen wurden zwischen den Kontrollbedingungen (1-Back und Go-Trials) und zwischen den eigentlichen Bedingungen der Arbeitsgedächtnis- und Stoppsignal-Aufgabe (2-Back und Stopperfolge) berechnet. Ergebnisse: Kontrollen und Patienten zeigten hirnphysiologisch aufgabentypische signifikante Aktivierungsmuster, für das Arbeitsgedächtnis mehr ausgeprägt über dem dorso-lateralen präfrontalen Kortex (DLPFC), für die Antworthemmung mehr über dem inferioren frontalen Kortex (IFC). Fragestellung 1: Patienten zeigten im Vergleich zu den gesunden Kontrollpersonen signifikante Defizite in allen erhobenen behavioralen Maßen. Sie zeigten auch signifikant reduzierte Anstiege von O2Hb über dem linken und rechten IFC für Stopperfolge und eine statistisch tendenziell verringerte O2Hb-Konzentration im linken DLPFC für die 2-Back-Bedingung. Es fanden sich jedoch auch Defizite in den einfachen Kontrollaufgaben (behaviorale Maße und O2Hb während 1-Back). Nach einer Anpassung der Gruppen für die Unterschiede in den Kontrollaufgaben blieben die Gruppenunterschiede in den Aufgaben für Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung bestehen. Fragestellung 2: Nur bei den Patienten und nur für die behavioralen Maße zeigten sich signifikante positive Assoziationen. Die Effizienz während 1-Back korrelierte mit der Reaktionszeit während der Go-Trials, und die Effizienz während 2-Back korrelierte mit der Stoppsignal-Reaktionszeit. Diese beiden Korrelationen unterschieden sich statistisch nicht in ihrer Stärke. In den für die Unterschiede in den Kontrollaufgaben angepassten Gruppen blieb nur die Korrelation der Kontrollaufgaben tendenziell signifikant, die Korrelation von Effizienz während 2-Back und Stoppsignal-Reaktionszeit verschwand. Die Patienten zeigten erhöhte Impulsivität (erhoben mit einem Fragebogen), die positiv mit behavioralen Maßen der beiden untersuchten Prozesse korrelierte. Fragestellung 3: Für 1-Back (O2Hb in rechter Hemisphäre) und 2-Back (O2Hb in linker Hemisphäre) wurden statistisch tendenzielle COMT x Gruppe Interaktionen gefunden. Für 1-Back zeigten die Kontrollen mit dem Val/Val-Genoytp sowohl im Vergleich zu Kontrollen mit dem Met/Met-Genotyp als auch im Vergleich zu Patienten mit zwei Val-Allelen eine signifikant höhere O2Hb-Konzentration. Gleichzeitig wies die Analyse der Verhaltensdaten auf eine höhere Effizienz der Kontrollen mit Met-Allelen im Vergleich zu Patienten mit Met-Allelen hin. Für 2-Back zeigte der Val/Val-Genotyp der Kontrollen eine höhere O2Hb-Konzentration als der Val/Val-Genotyp der Patienten. Zusätzlich wies der Met/Met-Genotyp in der ADHS-Gruppe einen stärkeren Anstieg von O2Hb im Vergleich zu dem Val/Val-Genotyp in der ADHS-Gruppe auf. Die Analyse der Verhaltensdaten zeigte eine höhere Effizienz der Kontrollen im Vergleich zu den Patienten für die Gruppen mit einem oder zwei Val-Allelen. Für Stopperfolge zeigten sich signifikante COMT x Gruppe Interaktionen für O2Hb in beiden Hemisphären. Die Kontrollen mit Val/Met-Genotyp hatten höhere O2Hb-Konzentrationen als Kontrollen mit Val/Val- oder Met/Met-Genotyp und als Patienten mit Val/Met-Genotyp. Patienten mit Val/Met-Genotyp hatten langsamere Stoppsignal-Reaktionszeiten im Vergleich zu den Kontrollen mit Val/Met-Genotyp. Fragestellung 4: Die Korrelationen in Abhängigkeit der Stichproben und COMT-Gruppen zeigten unterschiedlich hohe und unterschiedlich gerichtete Korrelationskoeffizienten. Nur in der Gruppe der Patienten und nur für die Verhaltensdaten wurden positive Assoziationen für alle COMT-Gruppen gefunden, vergleichbar mit den Ergebnissen aus Fragestellung 2. Diskussion: Die behavioralen und hirnphysiologischen Unterschiede in den Aufgaben zum Arbeitsgedächtnis und zur Antworthemmung zwischen Patienten mit ADHS und Kontrollen replizieren gut die Ergebnisse früherer Arbeiten. Ausgehend von den Gruppenunterschieden in den einfachen Kontrollaufgaben muss aber überlegt werden, inwieweit Patienten mit ADHS auch in einfachen psychomotorischen Aufgaben bereits Defizite zeigen. Die Korrelation behavioraler Maße der eigentlichen Arbeitsgedächtnis- und Inhibitionsaufgabe in der Gruppe der Patienten, die allerdings die Assoziation der Kontrollaufgaben nicht überschritt, und die mit dem Anpassen für Unterschiede in den Kontrollenaufgaben verschwand, lässt Zweifel an dem spezifischen Zusammenhang der beiden Prozesse bei ADHS aufkommen. Zudem werfen die positiven Assoziationen dieser Prozesse (behaviorale Maße) mit der Impulsivität (Fragebogen) die Frage auf, ob es sich um die Operationalisierung des gleichen Konstrukts handelt bzw. ob Impulsivität in Form behavioraler Tests das Gleiche misst wie Impulsivität auf symptomatischer Ebene. Ein Faktor, der möglicherweise Einfluss auf die Korrelationen zwischen den Aufgaben hat, könnte der Dopamingehalt sein, wie er in der vorliegenden Arbeit durch den COMT Polymorphismus operationalisiert wurde. Die Ergebnisse der COMT x Gruppe Interaktionen zeigten eine erhöhte O2Hb-Konzentration der Val-Allelträger der Kontrollen für die N-Back-Aufgaben bei teilweise besserer Effizienz und eine erhöhte O2Hb-Konzentration und teilweise schnellere Stoppsignal-Reaktionszeit der Val/Met-Allelträger der Kontrollen bei Stopperfolgen. Unsere Ergebnisse entsprechen nicht unbedingt den Annahmen aus der Literatur, dass das Met-Allel das günstige Allel in Bezug auf gute präfrontale Hirnfunktion sei. Diese Unterschiede in den COMT x Gruppe Interaktionen legen eine Abhängigkeit präfrontaler Hirnfunktionen vom allgemeinen dopaminergen Gehalt (Patienten vs. Kontrollen), vom präfrontalen dopaminergen Niveau (COMT-Genotyp) und von der Aufgabenart (Arbeitsgedächtnis vs. Antworthemmung) nahe. Diese Hypothese wird unterstützt durch die Unterschiede in den Korrelationskoeffizienten der beiden Prozesse in Abhängigkeit der Stichproben und Genotyp-Gruppen. Schlussfolgerung: Die Befunde stützen nicht die Annahme, dass es sich bei Arbeitsgedächtnis und Antworthemmung um zusammenhängende Funktionen bei ADHS oder Gesunden handelt, oder dass der exekutive Endophänotyp einen einheitlichen Subtyp der ADHS darstellt. Zudem scheint eine mögliche Assoziation der beiden Prozesse von mehreren Faktoren wie z. B. der Dopamin-Konzentration moduliert zu werden. Zukünftige Arbeiten sollten die Fragestellungen bearbeiten, welche Faktoren den Zusammenhang dieser Prozesse beeinflussen und welche Kombination solcher Faktoren eine Subgruppe von Patienten mit ADHS mit einem gemeinsamen exekutiven Endophänotyp darstellen könnten. / Objective: The highly heritable psychiatric disease Attention Deficit-/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is characterized by symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention. The pathophysiology seems to involve alterations in the dopaminergic neurotransmitter system as well as changes in the prefrontal cortex. Working memory and response inhibition are two executive functions that have repeatedly been found to be deficient in ADHD patients, and have therefore been proposed as potential endophenotypes of the disease. Such so called intermediate phenotypes are thought to be more closely related to aetiopathogenetic factors than subtypes defined according to the classical diagnostic systems. Many studies have shown mean group differences for working memory and response inhibition in ADHD samples as compared to healthy control groups, both with respect to behavioural and brain imaging variables. A genetic polymorphism (Val158Met) in the gene coding for the enzyme catechol O methyltransferase (COMT) modulates working memory and response inhibition functions in controls and patients with ADHD. COMT catalyzes the degradation of prefrontal dopamine. The valine(val-)allele is associated with a low, the methionine(met-)allele with a high level of dopamine in cortical areas. The met-allele seems to be beneficial for prefrontal functions. Aim: It is an open question, if the two processes of working memory and response inhibition are associated, i. e. if they characterize the same endophenotype, and if a possible association of these processes is modulated by dopamine. The present work aims at (1) replicating previously shown mean group differences, (2) analyzing the association of working memory and response inhibition in a correlational analyses, (3) investigating the influence of COMT on working memory and response inhibition, and (4) measuring the influence of COMT on the strength of the correlations between the two processes of working memory and response inhibition. Questions 3 and 4 are indicated as exploratory analyses due to small sample sizes. Methods: 45 adult patients with ADHD und 41 healthy controls were included in the analyses. The two groups were comparable for age, gender, handedness, intelligence, education level, and head perimeter. Working memory and response inhibition were operationalized by an n-back and a stop signal task. Behavioural dependent variables were the number of correct answers, reaction times, and efficiency. Dependent physiological parameters were concentration changes of oxygenated (O2Hb) and deoxygenated haemoglobin (HHb) in prefrontal cortical areas as assessed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Correlations were calculated between control conditions (1-back und go-trials) and between the active conditions of the working memory and response inhibition tasks (2-back und successful stop signals). Results: Controls and patients showed task-specific significant activation patterns, for working memory over dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and for response inhibition over inferior frontal cortex (IFC). Question 1: Compared to the control sample, patients had deficits in all behavioural parameters. They also showed significantly reduced increases in O2Hb in bilateral IFC for successful stop trials, and a statistical trend for a reduced increase in the left DLPFC for the 2-back condition. Furthermore, deficits were found in the simple control tasks (behavioural parameters and O2Hb during 1-back). After matching of the groups for differences in control tasks, group differences remained significant for tasks of working memory and response inhibition. Question 2: Significant positive associations were found only for the patients and only for behavioural variables. Efficiency during 1-back correlated with reaction time during go-trials, and efficiency during 2-back correlated with stop signal reaction time, with no statistical difference between the strength of these correlations. Only the correlation of the control tasks remained significant as a trend after matching of the groups for differences in the control tasks. The significant correlation of efficiency during 2-back und stop signal reaction time disappeared. Patients showed an elevated impulsivity (assessed by a questionnaire) correlating positively with behavioural measures of both tasks. Question 3: For the 1-back (O2Hb and right hemisphere) and 2-back condition (O2Hb and left hemisphere) COMT x group interactions were found as a statistical trend. For 1-back, controls with the val/val-genotype had higher oxygenation levels than controls with the met/met-genotype. Controls with val-alleles had higher O2Hb concentration changes than patients with val-alleles. Analysis of the behavioural data revealed higher efficiency for controls with met-alleles in contrast to patients with met-alleles. For 2-back, controls with the val/val-genotype showed higher oxygenation than val/val-allele carriers within the group of patients. Additionally, the met/met-genotype in the ADHD group had higher O2Hb changes than the val/val-genotype in the ADHD group. Behavioural analyses indicated higher efficiency in controls compared to patients for the groups with one or two val-alleles. For successful stop signals, significant COMT x group interactions were found for O2Hb in both hemispheres. Controls with the val/met-genotype had higher oxygenation than controls with the val/val- or met/met-genotype, and than patients with the val/met-genotype. Patients with the val/met-genotype had slower stop signal reaction times as compared to controls with the val/met-genotype. Question 4: Correlations showed differences in strength and direction depending on samples and COMT groups. Only for the patients and only for behavioral variables significant positive associations were found for all COMT groups, comparable to the results of question 2. Discussion: The behavioural and physiological differences observed between patients with ADHD and controls replicate previous findings. Considering differences in control tasks, it is an important issue for future research to investigate group differences in simple psychomotor tasks. The correlation for behavioural measures of working memory and response inhibition in the group of patients, which did not exceed the association of behavioural measures for the control tasks, and which disappeared after matching of groups for differences in control tasks, raises doubts regarding the hypothesis of a specific association of these two processes in ADHD. Considering the correlation of these processes (behavioural measures) with impulsivity (questionnaire), the question arises, whether impulsivity in terms of behavioural task measures reflects the same construct as impulsivity at a symptomatic level. One factor that might potentially influence correlations between tasks could be the level of extracellular prefrontal dopamine, as indicated by the COMT polymorphism in the present investigation. Results of the COMT x group interactions revealed an increased oxygenation and to some extent increased efficiency of the controls with val-allels for the n-back tasks and an increased oxygenation and to some extent a fastened stop signal reaction time of the controls with val/met-genotype. Our results do not correspond to the assumptions from literature indicating an association of the met allele with beneficial prefrontal functions. Differences in COMT x group interactions suggest that prefrontal functions depend on general dopaminergic level (patients vs. controls), on prefrontal dopaminergic level (COMT genotype), and on task type (working memory vs. response inhibition). This hypothesis is supported by the differences in correlations of both processes in dependence of sample and genotype group. Conclusion: Our findings do not directly support the assumption, that working memory and response inhibition are associated functions in patients with ADHD or controls, or that the executive phenotype is one integrative subtype of ADHD. Furthermore, a possible association of both functions seems to depend on several factors, e. g. dopaminergic level as shown in the current investigation. Future investigations should address the open questions as to which factors influence this association and what combinations of such factors might constitute a subgroup of patients with ADHD with an integrative executive endophenotype.
2

Der Kernbereich exekutiver Eigenverantwortung: Informationsansprüche des Parlaments gegenüber der Regierung unter Berücksichtigung des Grundsatzes der Gewaltenteilung

Perl, Robert 06 February 2023 (has links)
Das parlamentarische Recht zur Kontrolle der Regierungsarbeit ist ein fundamentaler Ausfluss des Demokratieprinzips. Der Grundsatz der Gewaltenteilung wiederum verlangt nach einer Begrenzung dieses Kontrollrechts. Gewisse Teile der Regierungsarbeit müssen auch langfristig vor dem Parlament verborgen bleiben – Doch wo verläuft die Grenze zwischen parlamentarischem Kontrollrecht und dem sog. Kernbereich exekutiver Eigenverantwortung?
3

Development of Inhibitory Control in Kindergarten Children / Entwicklung von Inhibitionskontrolle bei Kindern im Kindergartenalter

Schulz, Daniel January 2024 (has links) (PDF)
This dissertation explores the development and assessment of inhibitory control – a crucial component of executive functions – in young children. Inhibitory control, defined as the ability to suppress inappropriate responses (Verbruggen & Logan, 2008), is essential for adaptable and goal-oriented behavior. The rapid and non-linear development of this cognitive function in early childhood presents unique challenges for accurate assessment. As children age, they often exhibit a ceiling effect in terms of response accuracy (Petersen et al., 2016), underscoring the need to consider response latency as well. Ideally, combining response latency with accuracy could yield a more precise measure of inhibitory control (e.g., Magnus et al., 2019), facilitating a detailed tracking of developmental changes in inhibitory control across a wider age spectrum. The three studies of this dissertation collectively aim to clarify the relationship between response accuracy, response latency, and inhibitory control across different stages of child development. Each study utilizes a computerized Pointing Stroop Task (Berger et al., 2000) to measure inhibitory control, examining the task's validity and the integration of dual metrics for a more comprehensive evaluation. The first study focuses on establishing the validity of using both response accuracy and latency as indicators of inhibitory control. Utilizing the framework of explanatory item-response modeling (De Boeck & Wilson, 2004), the study revealed how the task characteristics congruency and item position influence both the difficulty level and timing aspects in young children’s responses in the computerized Pointing Stroop task. Further, this study found that integrating response accuracy with latency, even in a basic manner, provides additional insights. Building upon these findings, the second study investigates the nuances of integrating response accuracy and latency, examining whether this approach can account for age-related differences in inhibitory control. It also explores whether response latencies may contain different information depending on the age and proficiency of the children. The study leverages novel and established methodological perspectives to integrate response accuracy and latency into a single metric, showing the potential applicability of different approaches for assessing inhibitory control development. The third study extends the investigation to a longitudinal perspective, exploring the dynamic relationship between response accuracy, latency, and inhibitory control over time. It assesses whether children who achieve high accuracy at an earlier age show faster improvement in response latency, suggesting a non-linear maturation pathway of inhibitory control. The study also examines if the predictive value of early response latency for later fluid intelligence is dependent on the response accuracy level. Together, these empirical studies contribute to a more robust understanding of the complex interaction between inhibitory control, response accuracy, and response latency, facilitating valid evaluations of cognitive capabilities in children. Moreover, the findings may have practical implications for designing educational strategies and clinical interventions that address the developmental trajectory of inhibitory control. The nuanced approach advocated in this dissertation suggests prioritizing accuracy in assessment and interventions during the early stages of children's cognitive development, gradually shifting the focus to response latency as children mature and secure their inhibitory control abilities. / Die vorliegende Dissertation erforscht die Erfassung und Entwicklung von Inhibitionskontrolle bei jungen Kindern – einer zentralen Komponente der Exekutiven Funktionen. Inhibitionskontrolle, also die Fähigkeit, automatisierte aber unangemessene Reaktionen zu unterdrücken (Verbruggen & Logan, 2008), ist wesentlich für adaptives und zielgerichtetes Verhalten. Die schnelle und nichtlineare Entwicklung dieser kognitiven Funktion im frühen Kindesalter gestaltet eine präzise Messung herausfordernd. Mit zunehmendem Alter der Kinder zeigt sich häufig ein Deckeneffekt hinsichtlich der Antwortgenauigkeit (Petersen et al., 2016), was die Notwendigkeit hervorhebt, auch die Reaktionszeit in Betracht zu ziehen. Idealerweise könnte durch die Integration von Reaktionszeit und Antwortgenauigkeit ein Messwert berechnet werden (z.B. Magnus et al., 2019), welcher eine detaillierte Erfassung von Entwicklungsveränderungen der Inhibitionskontrolle über ein breiteres Altersspektrum hinweg ermöglicht. Die drei Studien dieser Dissertation zielen darauf ab, die Beziehung zwischen Antwortgenauigkeit, Reaktionszeit und Inhibitionskontrolle in verschiedenen Stadien der kindlichen Entwicklung zu untersuchen. Jede Studie nutzt eine computergestützte Inhibitionsaufgabe, den computerized Pointing-Stroop Task (cPST; Berger et al., 2000), um die Inhibitionskontrolle zu messen, wobei die Validität dieses Tests und die Integration von Antwortgenauigkeit und Reaktionszeit für eine umfassendere Bewertung untersucht werden. In der ersten Studie wird untersucht, ob sowohl Antwortgenauigkeit als auch Reaktionszeit valide Indikatoren für Inhibitionskontrolle in jungen Kindern darstellen. Unter Verwendung von explanatorischen Item-Response-Modellen zeigte die Studie, wie die Aufgabenmerkmale Kongruenz und Item-Position die Aufgabenschwierigkeit sowohl in Bezug auf Antwortgenauigkeit als auch Reaktionszeit im cPST beeinflussen. Darüber hinaus zeigten sich erste Hinweise, dass bereits eine rudimentäre Integration von Antwortgenauigkeit und Reaktionszeit zusätzliche Einsichten liefert. Aufbauend auf diesen Erkenntnissen untersucht die zweite Studie die Feinheiten der Integration von Antwortgenauigkeit und Reaktionszeit und prüft, ob moderne Methoden der Integration dieser beiden Metriken altersbedingte Unterschiede in der Inhibitionskontrolle berücksichtigen können. Sie erforscht auch, ob sich aus den Reaktionszeiten in Inhibitionsaufgaben, abhängig vom Alter und Können der Kinder, unterschiedliche Schlussfolgerungen ziehen lassen. Die Studie nutzt neue und etablierte methodische Ansätze, um Antwortgenauigkeit und Reaktionszeit zu einer Metrik zu integrieren und zeigt die potenzielle Anwendbarkeit verschiedener Ansätze zur Bewertung der Entwicklung der Inhibitionskontrolle. Die dritte Studie erweitert die Untersuchung auf eine Längsschnittperspektive und erforscht die dynamische Beziehung zwischen Antwortgenauigkeit, Reaktionszeit und Inhibitionskontrolle im Laufe der Entwicklung. Sie betrachtet, ob Kinder, die in jüngerem Alter eine hohe Genauigkeit erreichen, eine schnellere Verbesserung in der Reaktionszeit zeigen. Die Studie untersucht weiter, ob der prädiktive Wert von Reaktionszeit für zukünftige fluide Intelligenz in Abhängigkeit zu der Antwortgenauigkeit steht. Zusammen tragen diese empirischen Arbeiten zu einem tieferen Verständnis der komplexen Interaktion zwischen Inhibitionskontrolle, Antwortgenauigkeit und Reaktionszeit bei und erleichtern valide Bewertungen dieser kognitiven Fähigkeiten bei Kindern. Darüber hinaus könnten die Ergebnisse praktische Implikationen für die Gestaltung von Interventionen haben, die sich mit dem Entwicklungsverlauf der Inhibitionskontrolle befassen. Der in dieser Dissertation vertretene Ansatz legt nahe, Antwortgenauigkeit bei der Bewertung und Interventionen während der frühen Phasen der kognitiven Entwicklung von Kindern zu priorisieren und den Fokus allmählich auf die Reaktionszeit zu verlagern, sobald Kinder ihre Inhibitionskontrolle festigen und ausbauen.
4

Chronic Stress, Executive Functioning, and their Methodological Assessment Challenges

Schmidt, Kornelius 03 March 2021 (has links)
The 21st century world is characterized by globalization, optimization, high performance demands, and continuous acceleration of processes. Not surprisingly, stress has steadily become more prevalent over the past years and has become a permanent challenge for many of us. Still we are always expected to perform at our best, and an impairment of cognitive performance can be devastating, particularly in the professional world. This ubiquity in daily life of chronic stress, cognitive demands, and their potential interactions was the motivation for this dissertation. Among cognitive processes executive functioning (EF) are of particular interest, as they represent set of fundamental cognitive abilities for mastering daily life. More precisely, EF is described as higher-order cognitive processes that control and coordinate complex cognitive tasks (Diamond, 2013; Miyake et al., 2000). The primary aim of this dissertation was to investigate the interplay between chronic stress and EF in detail. A three-year longitudinal cohort, the StressCog cohort, was established for this purpose. In order to understand the longitudinal interplay between chronic stress and EF, methodological foundations and cross-sectional matters had to be investigated first. As a result, this dissertation encompasses the following three studies: With classical laboratory designs, the estimation of generalizable and robust effects on the relationship between chronic stress and EF is impaired. Therefore, the StressCog study was set up to make use of internet-based data assessment within domestic environments. Study 1 addressed the feasibility of internet-based response time data, including the common concern that internet-based response time data collected in domestic environments is subject to increased data variability (Chetverikov & Upravitelev, 2016; Reips, 2002). To this end, performance measures collected in the laboratory were compared with measures collected in domestic environments. We found that a setting-related difference in the variability of conventional performance measures (i.e., response times and error rates), as well as diffusion model-based measures is only of small size. An overall increase of variance of approximately 5% was visible in domestic environments. However, as internet-based assessments allow for the recruitment of larger sample sizes (Reips, 2002) the loss of static power can be easily compensated. Therefore, the findings of Study 1 support the use of internet-based cognitive data collection in domestic environments. This formed a valuable basis regarding the collection and interpretation of the StressCog data used in Study 2 and Study 3. In order to get a broad picture of chronic stress the StressCog study aimed for multimethod assessment. The subjective (i.e., perceived) extent of chronic stress and conceptually related constructs were assessed by multiple self-reported measures. The objective (i.e., physiological) extent of chronic stress was assessed via hair cortisol concentration, which has been established as a widely accepted biological marker of chronic stress (E. Russell et al., 2012; Stalder & Kirschbaum, 2012). Surprisingly, however, multiple studies have shown that hair cortisol concentration is, at best, only weakly correlated with subjective chronic stress (Stalder et al., 2017; Staufenbiel et al., 2013; Weckesser et al., 2019). This lack of psychoendocrine covariance was addressed in Study 2 by investigating the construct validity of commonly used chronic stress instruments. A multidimensional item response theory approach was applied in order to display the overlap between items of commonly used measures of chronic stress, depressiveness, and neuroticism. A common latent scale covered the major amount of variance (40% to 48%). It appeared that the overlap of content is mainly based on fatigue, which is a core symptom of depressiveness. Similar to previously reported findings, the common latent scale displayed only a weak association with hair cortisol concentration. It can therefore be argued that items of self-reported chronic stress do not reflect the physiological (i.e., endocrine) aspect of chronic stress to a satisfactory extent. Thus, the results of Study 2 were a valuable basis for the interpretation of chronic stress data in Study 3. In Study 3, we investigated the cross-sectional association between chronic stress and EF. The study made use of data from the baseline assessment of the StressCog cohort (N = 514). Using structural equation modelling, we found no evidence for a meaningful association between chronic stress (i.e., self-reported measures and hair cortisol concentration) and a common latent EF factor. The results found in Study 3 were further supported by unpublished longitudinal StressCog data and analyses making use of diffusion model performance measures. Considering the mainstream of existing findings, which suggests chronic stress to be negatively associated with EF (e.g., Deligkaris et al., 2014; Sandi, 2013), an absence of an association between chronic stress and EF seems surprising. However, other existing findings support this perspective (Castaneda et al., 2011a; McLennan et al., 2016). Beyond this, the results of Study 1 and 2 help to explain why a lack of an association between chronic stress and EF is quite plausible. As within traditional, laboratory-based settings a recruitment of larger samples sizes is difficult and costly, most existing studies lack large, representative samples. In consequence, many findings are based on narrow sample characteristics. Supported by the findings of Study 1, the StressCog study made use of internet-based data collection, which led to the establishment of one the largest, population-based cohorts in the field. Thus, the results presented in this dissertation can be seen as much more robust and representative than many other studies that suggest opposing results. The findings of Study 2 allow for the assumption that many studies in the field make use of questionable self-report instruments and that subjective (i.e., self-reported) measures of chronic stress are only weakly related to objective (i.e., HCC) measures. In consequence, results always depend on the operationalization of the chronic stress measures being used. Caution is advised when comparing opposing results of different measures. With regard to our daily life demands, it may appear quite promising that no association between chronic stress and EF was found in this dissertation. However, these results should not be misunderstood. It has been demonstrated countless times that chronic stress can severely affect the human organism, causing various negative effects that go far beyond a possible impairment of cognitive abilities. Knowing that chronic stress can impair EF under certain circumstances leads to the central question of what conditions make stress toxic. In order to answer this question further systematic research is needed, in which representative samples and experimental study designs will be of crucial help. As long as these questions are not satisfactorily clarified, it seems best to deal with the challenges of chronic stress in a conscious and responsible manner.
5

Executive functions modulated by context, training, and age

Strobach, Tilo 16 April 2014 (has links)
Der vorliegende Arbeit systematisiert Befunde aus dem Gebiet der exekutiven Funktionen und ihren Modulationen durch verschiedene Parameter (d.h. Shifting, Inhibition, Updating, Dual tasking). Diese Parameter werden in einer entsprechenden Rahmenkonzeption illustriert und umfassen die Parameter Kontext, Training, Alter und ihre Kombinationen. Die vorliegende Arbeit zeigt Beispiele, wie diese Parameter in Bezug auf exekutive Funktionen realisiert werden können. Im Rahmen des Kontext-Parameters wird beispielsweise gezeigt, wie die Vorhersagbarkeit von Aufgaben und Aufgabenreihenfolgen Dual tasking (Heinrich et al., 2012) und wie aktuelle Anforderungen an das Arbeitsgedächtnis Inhibition (Soutschek et al., 2013) modulieren kann. Allerdings zeigen Töllner et al. (2012) auch Grenzen des Kontext-Parameters bei der Modulation der erstgenannten exekutiven Funktion. Der Parameter Training wurde realisiert mit Fokus auf (1) Shifting (Strobach et al., 2012a) und (2) Updating (Salminen et al., 2011, 2012). Schubert and Strobach (2012) sowie Strobach et al. (2012b) untersuchten den Transfereffekt von Video-game- und Arbeitsgedächtnistraining auf Dual tasking und Shifting. Schließlich zeigen Strobach et al. (2012c, 2012d) Modulationseffekte bei der Kombination des Alters- und Trainingsparameters auf die exekutive Funktion Dual tasking. Abschließend systematisiert die vorgestellte Rahmenkonzeption die Lücken und Ziele zukünftiger Forschung zur Modulation von exekutiven Funktionen. / The present review aims at a systematization of findings in the field of executive functions and their modulation due to different operational parameters (i.e., dimensions). This systematization is realized in the form of the Framework on modulations of executive functions. Basically, this framework illustrates how different types of executive functions (i.e., Shifting, Inhibition, Updating, Dual tasking) are modulated by their context (Context dimension), training (Training dimension), age (Age dimension), and combinations of these dimensions. The present review includes examples of studies that demonstrate a realization of each of these dimensions and their effects on executive function types. In detail, the Context dimension modulates the executive function type Dual tasking (i.e., due to task order predictability in dual tasks; Hendrich et al., 2012 ) and Inhibition (i.e., due to the level of concurrent working memory demand in a Stroop task; Soutschek et al., 2013); however, Töllner et al. (2012) demonstrated that Context also affects processes that are not related with executive functioning (i.e., perception and motor processes) in a dual-task situation. The framework’s Training dimension was realized when (1) Strobach et al. (2012a) investigated effects of training on the executive function Shifting in a task switching situation and (2) Salminen et al. (2011, 2012), Schubert and Strobach (2012), as well as Strobach et al. (2012b) investigated transfer effects on executive functioning of Dual tasking and Shifting after video game and working memory training. Finally, Strobach et al. (2012c, 2012d) illustrated modulation effects of age and training (i.e., the combination of Age and Training dimension) on the executive function type Dual tasking. In sum, this framework helps to systematize research gaps and future studies in this field (i.e., executive functions).
6

Bewertung individueller Voraussetzungen zur Bewältigung wechselnder Arbeitsanforderungen anhand psychologischer und neuropsychologischer Parameter / Evaluation of individual conditions for the accomplishment of changing work requirements on the basis psychological and neurophysiological parameters

May, Jana January 2004 (has links)
Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit war es, mittels des Aufgabenwechselparadigmas, kognitive Prozesse nicht nur anhand von traditionellen Leistungsparametern, sondern zusätzlich durch elektro-physiologische Parameter zu untersuchen. Parameter ereigniskorrelierter Hirnpotentiale (EKP) wurden ebenfalls zur Einschätzung von altersbedingten Änderungen bei der Ausführung von Reaktionszeitaufgaben herangezogen.<br> Nach Rubinstein et al. (2001) setzt sich die Reaktionszeit aus der Dauer seriell angeordneter Verarbeitungsstufen zusammen. Im Stufenmodell der exekutiven Kontrolle von Rubinstein et al. (2001) sind Prozesse der ausführenden Kontrolle nur an Wechseltrials beteiligt und können getrennt von den Aufgabenprozessen ablaufen. Mittels der Informationen zu den Reaktionszeiten ist es jedoch nicht möglich zu klären, auf welche kognitiven Verarbeitungsprozesse Reaktionszeitunterschiede unter den jeweiligen experimentellen Bedingungen zurückzuführen sind. Zur Analyse der kognitiven Prozesse wurden in dieser Untersuchung die CNV und P300 herangezogen. Es wurden zwei Altersgruppen (20-30 Jährige und 49-61 Jährige) untersucht. Den Probanden wurden Ziffern präsentiert, die entweder nach dem numerischen Wert oder der Schriftgröße mit dem Hinweisreiz, der Zahl 5, verglichen werden sollten. Die Stimuli wurden nach dem Alternating-Runs-Paradigma dargeboten (Rogers und Monsell, 1995).<br> Erwartungsgemäß gab es Reaktionszeitunterschiede zwischen alt und jung mit längeren Reaktionszeiten für die älteren Probanden. Altersunterschiede in den Fehlerraten ließen sich nicht nachweisen. Möglicherweise erfolgte die Reaktionsauswahl bei den Älteren überlegter aus als bei den Jüngeren. Dies spiegelte sich in längeren aber fehlerfreien Reaktionen wider. Vermutlich bereiteten jedoch alle Probanden in dem Intervall zwischen Cue und Stimulus das jeweilige Aufgabenset komplett vor. Das könnte auch erklären, warum es bei einem Aufgabenwechsel nicht zu einem Anstieg der Reaktionszeit und der Fehlerrate kam. Entgegen der Erwartung zeigten sich keine Wechselkosten. Teilweise wurden inverse Wechselkosten nachgewiesen. In Bezug auf die Wechselkosten konnte das Stufenmodell der exekutiven Kontrolle (Rubinstein et al., 2001) nicht bestätigt werden. Der explizite Hinweisreiz scheint allerdings Einfluss auf die Wechselkosten zu haben. Verschiedene Erklärungsansätze werden diskutiert.<br> Die Contingent Negative Variation ist wie erwartet vor einem Aufgabenwechsel größer als vor einer Aufgabenwiederholung. Durch den Hinweisreiz ist eine erhöhte Kapazität vorhanden. Entsprechend den Ergebnissen der CNV kann davon ausgegangen werden, dass ältere Erwachsene stärker von der Vorinformation zu profitieren scheinen als jüngere Erwachsene. Die älteren Erwachsenen beginnen im Gegensatz zu den jüngeren Erwachsenen offenbar eher mit der Vorbereitung. Zeitdruck und Aufgabenwechsel lösen eine stärkere P300 aus. Demzufolge scheinen Zeitdruck und Aufgabenwechsel einen erhöhten Kapazitätsbedarf zu erfordern. Im Sinne des Stufenmodells der exekutiven Kontrolle von Rubinstein et al. (2001) führt die Zielverschiebung bei einem Aufgabenwechsel zu einer größeren P300. Die Resultate der hier dargestellten Untersuchungen verdeutlichen, dass ältere Erwachsene einen höheren zeitlichen Aufwand in den Stufen der einzelnen exekutiven Prozesse benötigen. Dies spricht für die Hypothese der selektiven Verlangsamung. Ältere kompensieren dies durch einen höheren Aufwand in der Vorbereitung, was auf elektrokortikaler Ebene nachweisbar ist, sind aber nicht in der Lage, dies in den Reaktionszeiten umzusetzen.<br> Die Ergebnisse dieser Untersuchung unterstützen die vereinfachte Annahme von Rubinstein et al. (2001), nach dem die Teilprozesse der Reaktionszeit seriell verarbeitet werden können. Die Resultate lassen allerdings den Schluss zu, dass die Wechselkosten im Hinblick auf die Reaktionszeiten nicht der geeignete Parameter für die Messung der exekutiven Kontrolle sind.<br> Die vorgeschlagene Modifikation des Modells von Rubinstein et al. (2001) in der Vorbereitung auf eine Aufgabe gilt es in weiteren Untersuchungen zu bestätigen und die Möglichkeit der Anwendung auf alle Aspekte der exekutiven Kontrollprozesse zu prüfen. / The aim of this study was it to examine cognitive processes not only on the basis achievement parameters by means of the alternating runs paradigm, but additionally by electricalphysiological parameters. Parameters of event-correlated brain potentials (EKP) were also used to estimate age-related changes in tasks of response time.<br> According to Rubinstein et al. (2001) the response time consists of the duration of serially arranged processing levels. In Rubinstein's et al. (2001) stage model of the executive control processes of implementing control are involved only in switch trials and can run separately from the task processes. The information from response times do not aloud to define what cognitive processing processes are responsible for response time differences in respective to the experimental conditions. In this study the contingent negative variation (CNV) and P300 were used for the analysis of the cognitive processes.<br> Two age groups (20 to 30, and 49 to 61 years old) were included in the study. Numbers were presented, which should be compared to the cue number 5 either to the numeric value or character size. The stimuli were represented after the alternating runs paradigm (Rogers & Monsell, 1995).<br> As expected there were response time differences between old and young subjects with longer response times for the older ones. Age differences in the error rates could not be proven. It is possible that the reaction selection in older ones took place with more consideration than in the younger ones. This is reflected in longer but error free reactions. Probably all subjects prepared in the interval the respective task set between cue and stimulus completely. This could also explain, why with a task switching no rise of the response time and the error rate was noticed. Against expectation no switch costs showed up. Inverse switch costs were partly proven. Regarding the switch costs the stage model of the executive control (Rubinstein et al., 2001) could not be confirmed. The explicit cue however seems to have influence on the switch costs. Different explanations are discussed.<br> The contingent negative variation is higher before a task switching than before a repetition of task. By the cue an increased capacity is presented. According to the results of the CNV it can be assumed that older adults seem to profit more strongly than younger adults from the advance information. Obviously the older adults begin earlier with the preparation than the younger ones. Time pressure and task switching release a stronger P300. Therefore time pressure and task switching seem to require an increased capacity need. According to the stage model of the executive control (Rubinstein et al., (2001) the goal shift with goes along with the task switching leads to a higher P300.<br> The results of the study represented here clarify, that older adults need a more time in the stages of the individual executives of processes. This underlines the hypothesis of the selective slowing down. Older ones compensate this by more effort to preparation, which can be proven on the electrocortical level. The are not able to show this in the the response times, though. The results of this study support the simplified acceptance of Rubinstein et al. (2001), after which the subprocesses of the response time can be processed serially.<br> The results permit the conclusion that the switch costs regarding the response times are not the adaquate parameter for the measurement of the executive control.<br> The suggested modification of the model of Rubinstein et al. (2001) in the preparation for a task it applies to be confirmed in further investigations and the possibility of application to all aspects of the executives has to be tested.
7

Plasticity of Executive Control Induced by Process-Based Cognitive Training Across the Life-Span

Zinke, Katharina 12 October 2012 (has links) (PDF)
Plasticity is a central concept within the life-span approach of development and is defined as the ability of an individual to change and reorganize in response to environmental challenges (e.g., Baltes & Singer, 20019. Such intraindividual changes can be induced by systematic cognitive training. Recent studies suggest that substantial amounts of plasticity can be induced in executive control functions with a process-based training approach. These newer studies show that repeated practice on executive control tasks not only improved performance on these trained tasks, but also led to improvements in nontrained tasks (i.e., transfer; e.g., Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Karbach & Kray, 2009). Executive control processes are especially relevant from a developmental perspective because executive control is involved in a wide range of complex cognitive activities (e.g., van der Sluis, de Jong, & van der Leij, 2007) and is one of the most central areas of cognitive development (e.g., Craik & Bialystok, 2006). The current thesis aimed at elucidating several important questions concerning the plasticity of executive control functions induced by systematic cognitive training. Firstly, the amount, range, and stability of plasticity in adolescents and older adults were investigated. Secondly, studies explored if training design, age, and interindividual differences moderate the amount and range of plasticity. Furthermore, the current thesis aimed at exploring how process-based training specifically leads to transfer effects. To explore these questions, all studies employed a pretest-posttest-design comparing a group of participants that was trained with a process-based training approach to a group of control partici-pants that did not receive the training. Pretraining and posttraining sessions incorporated systematic assessment of transfer measures in different cognitive domains. The first study set out to investigate if executive control can be trained in adolescents with a task switching training. Additionally, the study explored what particular domains of executive control may underlie training and transfer effects, and if acute bouts of exercise directly prior to cognitive training enhance training effects. Analyses indicated substantial training effects for both training groups (with or without acute exercise) and near transfer to a similar switching task. Other findings of transfer were limited to a speed task and a tendency for faster reaction times in an updating task. Thus, findings indicate, for the first time, that executive control can be enhanced in adolescents through a short training. Furthermore, analyses suggest that updating may be of particular relevance for the effects of the task switching training. Analyses revealed no additional effects of the exercise intervention. The second study set out to explore, for the first time, the effects of a process-based training ap-proach in old-old age (above 80 years). After ten sessions of practice on working memory tasks, the training group improved in four of the five trained tasks, emphasizing the potential for plasticity even in old-old age. The gains in the training group were largely driven by individuals who started out with a low capacity in the training tasks. Thus, findings suggest that working memory can be improved with a short executive control training even in old-old age, particularly for low-capacity individuals. The absence of transfer effects in this study may point to the limits of plasticity in this age group. The third study aimed at further elucidating the mixed findings regarding the amounts of training and transfer effects induced by executive control training in older adults. For that purpose, a sample of older adults covering a wide range from young-old to old-old age (65 to 95 years) was either trained for nine sessions on a visuospatial and a verbal working memory as well as an executive control task; or served as controls. Analyses revealed significant training effects in all three trained tasks, as well as near transfer to verbal working memory and far transfer to a nonverbal reasoning task. Remarkably, all training effects and the transfer effect to verbal working memory were even stable at a nine-month follow-up. These findings suggest that cognitive plasticity is preserved over a large range of old age and that even a rather short training regimen can lead to (partly specific) training and transfer effects. However, analyses also revealed that there are a range of factors that may moderate the amount of plasticity, e.g., age and baseline performance in the training domain. To summarize, the current thesis explored effects of short executive control trainings on cognitive functions in adolescents and older adults. The findings suggest a high potential for intraindividual variability across the whole life-span. Plasticity was shown on the level of training and transfer tasks, as well as on the level of stability of effects. Furthermore, results support the notion that process-based training improves executive control processes that in turn lead to improvements in tasks that rely on these processes. The current thesis makes important contributions to the conceptual debate about the potentials and limits of training-induced plasticity across the life-span. It benefits the debate in that it specifically delineates factors that moderate the obtained effects.
8

Potentials and Limits of Plasticity Induced by Working Memory Training in Old-Old Age

Zinke, Katharina, Zeintl, Melanie, Eschen, Anne, Herzog, Carole, Kliegel, Matthias 21 February 2014 (has links) (PDF)
Background: Old-old age (80+ years) is associated with substantial cognitive decline. In this population, training-induced cognitive plasticity has rarely been studied. While earlier findings on strategy trainings suggested reduced training gains in old-old age, recent results of an extensive process-based working memory (WM) training have been more positive. Objective: Following up on previous research, the present study aimed at examining the effects of a short WM training in old-old adults and the influence of baseline WM capacity on training gains. Methods: A training group (mean age: 86.8 years) and a matched control group (mean age: 87.1 years) participated in the study. The WM training consisted of five tasks that were trained in each of 10 sessions. To evaluate possible transfer effects, executive functions were assessed with two tests before and after training. The training group was divided via median split in high- and low-capacity individuals to determine the influence of baseline WM capacity on training gains. Results: The training group improved in four of the trained tasks (medium-to-large effects). Training gains were significantly larger in the training group than in the control group in only two of those tasks. The training effects were mainly driven by the low-capacity individuals who improved in all trained tasks. No transfer effects were observed. Conclusions: These positive effects of a short WM training, particularly for low-capacity individuals, emphasize the potential for cognitive plasticity in old-old age. The absence of transfer effects may also point to its limits. / Dieser Beitrag ist mit Zustimmung des Rechteinhabers aufgrund einer (DFG-geförderten) Allianz- bzw. Nationallizenz frei zugänglich.
9

Potentials and Limits of Plasticity Induced by Working Memory Training in Old-Old Age

Zinke, Katharina, Zeintl, Melanie, Eschen, Anne, Herzog, Carole, Kliegel, Matthias January 2012 (has links)
Background: Old-old age (80+ years) is associated with substantial cognitive decline. In this population, training-induced cognitive plasticity has rarely been studied. While earlier findings on strategy trainings suggested reduced training gains in old-old age, recent results of an extensive process-based working memory (WM) training have been more positive. Objective: Following up on previous research, the present study aimed at examining the effects of a short WM training in old-old adults and the influence of baseline WM capacity on training gains. Methods: A training group (mean age: 86.8 years) and a matched control group (mean age: 87.1 years) participated in the study. The WM training consisted of five tasks that were trained in each of 10 sessions. To evaluate possible transfer effects, executive functions were assessed with two tests before and after training. The training group was divided via median split in high- and low-capacity individuals to determine the influence of baseline WM capacity on training gains. Results: The training group improved in four of the trained tasks (medium-to-large effects). Training gains were significantly larger in the training group than in the control group in only two of those tasks. The training effects were mainly driven by the low-capacity individuals who improved in all trained tasks. No transfer effects were observed. Conclusions: These positive effects of a short WM training, particularly for low-capacity individuals, emphasize the potential for cognitive plasticity in old-old age. The absence of transfer effects may also point to its limits. / Dieser Beitrag ist mit Zustimmung des Rechteinhabers aufgrund einer (DFG-geförderten) Allianz- bzw. Nationallizenz frei zugänglich.
10

Understanding age-related prospective memory performance: The role of cognitive, motivational and emotional mechanisms associated with age differences in the delayed execution of intended actions

Schnitzspahn, Katharina 30 August 2011 (has links) (PDF)
A pervasive real-world memory task is remembering to carry out intended activities at appropriate moments in the future, such as remembering to call one’s mother after returning from work or to hand a message to a colleague when seeing him in the office on the next morning. Such types of tasks are termed prospective memory (PM) tasks (Einstein & McDaniel, 1996). PM has been identified as one of the most frequent everyday memory challenges (e.g., Maylor, 1990), particularly in old age (McDaniel, Einstein, & Rendell, 2008) and an intact PM is considered to be crucial for the maintenance of independent living (Kliegel & Martin, 2003). Therefore, many researchers have focused on the exploration of possible age differences in PM. While age-related deficits were found in standard lab-based PM tasks, age-related benefits occured in naturalistic tasks that are carried out in participants’ everyday lives. This surprising pattern has been called the age-PM-paradox (Rendell & Craik, 2000). It has been supported by a meta-analysis comparing PM age effects found in studies that focused either on lab-based or on naturalistic PM tasks (Henry, MacLeod, Phillips, & Crawford, 2004). However, the mechanisms which are critical in determining the direction of age effects remain poorly delineated. Thus, the overall aim of the research programme presented in the present thesis was to investigate the age-PM-paradox as well as potential cognitive, motivational and emotional mechanisms and processes associated with age-related PM performance. For that purpose, three experimental studies were conducted testing adult age effects in different PM task settings with different task material. Furthermore, several possible underlying mechanisms suggested by the literature on age effects in PM were measured and/ or varied experimentally. The first aim of Study 1 was to cross-validate the age-PM-paradox within a single sample. The second aim was to empirically explore the relative importance of four recently proposed factors (motivation, metacognitive awareness, activity absorption, and control over the task) that may be associated with the direction of age effects inside and outside of the laboratory. For that purpose, 20 young and 20 older adults performed a lab-based and a naturalistic PM task, which were similar in structure and demand. The level of control was experimentally manipulated in both task settings. The remaining possibly influencing factors (motivation, metacognitive awareness, and activity absorption) were assessed via questionnaires in the laboratory and with a daily diary in the field. First, analysing mean level age differences, the paradox was confirmed. Second, exploring possible correlates of the paradox revealed that the level of daily activity absorption (i.e., everyday stress) was the most important mechanism in naturalistic PM performance. Further, high motivation and good metacognitive awareness were associated with age benefits in PM performance in the naturalistic task, while high ongoing activity absorption and low control over the PM cue were related to deficits in lab-based tasks. Thus, Study 1 confirmed the age-PM-paradox within one sample and with carefully matched lab-based and naturalistic tasks. In addition, the results indicate that the relative importance of the suggested factors may vary as a function of setting. While cognitive factors were most influential in the laboratory, motivational and knowledge-based factors were associated with high PM performance in the naturalistic task. The strong association between PM performance in the field and everyday stress highlights the need for future studies exploring the mechanisms underlying this effect. Results from Study 1 suggest that cognitive resources are most influential for PM age effects in the laboratory. Yet, it is not clear, which specific cognitive resources are needed for successful PM performance and if these processes differ between young and older adults. Thus, Study 2 explored the role of executive functions (i.e. shifting, updating and inhibition) as possible developmental mechanisms associated with PM age effects. 170 young and 110 older adults performed a battery of cognitive tests including measures of PM, shifting, updating, inhibition, working memory and speed. A comprehensive set of statistical approaches (e.g. median analyses, structural equation modelling) was used to analyze the possible cognitive correlates in predicting PM performance. First, age effects were confirmed in PM and also obtained in measures of executive control. Moreover, the facets of executive control differently predicted PM performance. Specifically, shifting was the strongest predictor of PM performance in young and older adults as well as for explaining age differences in PM. Thus, Study 2 clarified the role of different facets of controlled attention in age effects in PM and bears important conceptual implications: The results suggest that executive functions are important developmental mechanisms of PM across adulthood beyond working memory and speed. Specifically, shifting appeared to be an essential aspect of cognitive control involved in age-related PM performance. Moreover, examining PM as a latent construct confirmed the convergent and discriminant validity of PM. This demonstrates PM as a separate cognitive construct and suggests that PM is related to, but not identical with, executive control. Study 3 was set out to explore if the amount of cognitive resources needed to successfully perform a PM task in the laboratory can be influenced by the emotionality of the task material. First studies suggested that emotional task material may enhance PM performance in young and older adults by heightening the salience of the task and thereby reducing the need for controlled attention. However, the extent and mechanisms of this effect are still under debate. Therefore, Study 3 explored possible differential effects of PM target cue valence on PM age effects. For that purpose, 45 young and 41 older adults performed a PM task in which emotional valence of the PM cue was manipulated (positive, negative, neutral). Results revealed an interaction indicating that age differences were smaller in both emotional valence conditions compared to the neutral condition. This finding supports an emotionally enhanced memory effect in PM, but only for the older adults as PM performance in young adults was not affected by cue valence. From a conceptual perspective, the results from Study 3 may also contribute to the explanation of the age-PM-paradox, as they suggest that the neutral material usually applied in lab-based studies might overestimate PM age effects. In summary, the present thesis makes an important contribution to the ongoing conceptual debate concerning adult age effects in PM performance assessed in the laboratory versus participants’ everyday lives. Results strongly suggest that mostly different variables may be crucial for understanding PM age deficits in the laboratory and age benefits in naturalistic PM tasks. Successful PM performance in the laboratory seems to require high levels of cognitive resources. The present results suggest that shifting ability is especially relevant in this respect. On a task level the emotionality of the material seems to influence the required amount of cognitive resources as it reduced PM age effects. Everyday stress seems to be particularly important for successful PM performance in the field. Thus, possible future studies should specify the relation between stress and PM as outlined in the general discussion.

Page generated in 0.0309 seconds