• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 172
  • 35
  • 22
  • 16
  • 14
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 8
  • 7
  • 7
  • 5
  • 4
  • 4
  • Tagged with
  • 352
  • 81
  • 58
  • 35
  • 35
  • 34
  • 23
  • 21
  • 19
  • 18
  • 17
  • 17
  • 17
  • 16
  • 16
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
301

Étude en laboratoire du rêve de personnes atteintes de schizophrénie

Lusignan, Félix-Antoine 12 1900 (has links)
La schizophrénie découle d’une modification du fonctionnement du cerveau et entraîne divers symptômes qui ont pour conséquence une altération de la perception de la réalité, des troubles cognitifs, et des dysfonctionnements sociaux et comportementaux. En plus des observations cliniques de jour, la schizophrénie montre également des signes nocturnes qui peuvent se manifester durant le rêve. Cette thèse vise à caractériser les rêves dans la schizophrénie et cherche à explorer les relations qui existent entre les caractéristiques du contenu onirique des personnes atteintes de schizophrénie et les symptômes de cette maladie. Pour ce faire, nous avons comparé diverses mesures de l’activité onirique recueillies auprès de 14 participants atteints de schizophrénie traités sous antipsychotiques atypiques et 15 participants témoins par le biais de questionnaires et de collectes de rêves en laboratoire à la suite d’éveils provoqués en sommeil paradoxal (SP) et en sommeil lent (SL). Les résultats obtenus au questionnaire révèlent que les participants atteints de schizophrénie rapportent un nombre de cauchemars plus élevé comparativement aux participants témoins. Les collectes en laboratoire démontrent une fréquence de rappel de rêves équivalente au sein des deux groupes de participants, indépendamment du stade de sommeil durant lequel elles sont effectuées. Les récits de rêves du SL des deux groupes de participants sont généralement plus courts et comprennent un nombre moins élevé d’items quantifiables comparativement à ceux du SP. Les récits de rêves recueillis en SP et en SL chez les participants atteints de schizophrénie s’avèrent plus courts que ceux des participants témoins et, lorsque le nombre de mots est pondéré, la plupart des différences observées dans le contenu de rêve entre les deux groupes tendent à disparaître. En comparaison aux participants témoins, ceux atteints de schizophrénie évaluent leurs rêves comme étant moins bizarres, en dépit d’un nombre équivalent d’éléments bizarres dans leurs récits. Finalement, bien qu’il n’y ait pas de différence dans la densité des mouvements oculaires rapides (MORs) entre les deux groupes de participants, seuls les participants témoins montrent une corrélation positive entre les MORs et certaines variables du contenu onirique. Les résultats de la présente thèse suggèrent que les caractéristiques du contenu onirique des personnes atteintes de schizophrénie peuvent refléter certaines des manifestations psychopathologiques de cette maladie. / Schizophrenia is a chronic, severe, and disabling brain disease which is characterized by symptoms which cause altered reality perception, cognitive deficits, and impairment in social or vocational functioning. In addition to clinical symptoms, schizophrenia can be accompanied with nocturnal characteristics which could manifest during dreaming. Using both questionnaire-based measures and laboratory REM sleep and non-REM sleep awakenings, we sought to characterize the dream content of 14 participants with schizophrenia under atypical antipsychotic medication. Results were compared with those from 15 healthy individuals. The relationship between dream content and daytime functioning in schizophrenia was also explored. Questionnaire data revealed that when compared to controls, patients with schizophrenia report experiencing a greater number of nightmares. Laboratory awakenings revealed that there was no significant difference between the two groups in the number of dream reports with reportable content, regardless of the sleep stage in which dreams were collected. In addition, when compared to their REM dream counterparts, both groups’ non-REM dream reports were shorter and included significantly fewer reportable items on several content scales. Laboratory REM and non-REM dream narratives from the patients were shorter and, after controlling for report length, most significant differences in dream content between the two groups disappeared. Patients with schizophrenia spontaneously rated their dream reports as being less bizarre than did controls, despite a similar density of bizarre elements as scored by external judges. Finally, both groups had a comparable density of rapid eye movements during REM sleep but a significant positive correlation between eye-movement density and dream content variables was only found in controls. Taken together, these findings suggest that dream content characteristics in schizophrenia may reflect psychopathological parameters specific to this disorder.
302

Alexithymie et appauvrissement onirique chez des populations cliniques souffrant de troubles du sommeil

Levrier, Katia 02 1900 (has links)
Le premier objectif de cette étude était d’évaluer la relation entre l’alexithymie et différents troubles du sommeil chez des patients diagnostiqués (N= 580) selon la polysomnographie et la classification de l’American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) et chez des sujets contrôle (N= 145) en utilisant l’Échelle d’Alexithymie de Toronto à 20 items (TAS-20). Le deuxième objectif était d’estimer le lien entre l’alexithymie et des caractéristiques de rêves suivant un Questionnaire sur les Rêves de 14 items. Les résultats confirment un lien entre l’alexithymie et les troubles du sommeil. Sa prévalence était supérieure dans le groupe clinique comparativement au groupe contrôle, et était différente selon les troubles. Les hommes cotaient plus haut que les femmes à l’Échelle d’Alexithymie de Toronto à 20 items (TAS-20) et sur ses sous-échelles DDF (difficulty describing feeling) et EOT (externally oriented thinking). L’EOT pourrait être impliquée dans les troubles de sommeil en étant l’unique sous-échelle, où un effet principal des diagnostics était significatif dans le groupe clinique. Pour les rêves, le score du TAS-20 corrélait positivement avec le facteur « détresse des cauchemars »; et négativement avec « rappel de rêves » et « signification des rêves ». Les sous-échelles du TAS-20 avaient des corrélations différentes: positive entre DIF et « détresse des cauchemars », négative entre DDF et « rappel de rêves » et EOT avec « signification des rêves ». À part quelques exceptions, ces modèles sont obtenus pour les groupes cliniques et non-cliniques, et pour les hommes et les femmes dans ces deux groupes. Ces résultats suggèrent un modèle consistant, et reproductible, de relations entre l’alexithymie et les composantes des rêves. / Using a large clinical group of patients suffering from sleep disorders (N= 580) and non-clinical comparison subjects (N= 145), the first aim of this study was to evaluate the relationship between alexithymia and several different sleep disorders diagnosed from polysomnography following the American Academy of Sleep Medicine classification system and using a Canadian French translation of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia scale (TAS-20). The second objective was to evaluate the link between alexithymia and dream characteristics using a 14-item Dreaming Questionnaire. Results confirm a relationship between alexithymia and sleep disorders. Its prevalence was higher in the clinical than the non-clinical group and differed according to sleep disorder diagnoses. Men scored higher than women on the TAS-20 total score and on the DDF (difficulty describing feeling) and EOT (externally oriented thinking) subscales. EOT could be implicated in sleep disorders pathology as it was the only subscale for which the sleep diagnosis effect was significant in the clinical group. Concerning the second objective, TAS-20 total score correlated positively with nightmare distress and negatively with dream recall; and correlated negatively with dream meaning. TAS-20 subscales were differentially correlated with the 3 dream factors of the Dreaming Questionnaire: DIF with increased nightmare distress, DDF with decreased dream recall and EOT with decreased dream meaning. With some exceptions, these patterns were obtained for clinical and non-clinical groups and for men and women. These results suggest a consistent and replicable pattern of relationships between alexithymia and dreaming components.
303

Temporal patterns of memory source incorporations into dreams and their relationships to dreamed locus of control

Solomonova, Elizaveta 12 1900 (has links)
Les incorporations des mémoires épisodiques dans les rêves apparaissent en formes fragmentées et suivent un modèle temporel distinct qui suit une courbe sinusoïdale. Ce modèle est caractérisé par les incorporations immédiates, qui apparaissent 1-2 jours après l’événement (effet de résidus diurnes), et les incorporations tardives, qui apparaissent 5-7 jours après l’événement (effet de délai). Ces deux effets sont considérés comme des liens entre les processus de consolidation de la mémoire et la formation du rêve. Cette courbe temporelle a été observée pour une variété de stimuli expérimentaux. Cependant, aucune étude à date n’a démontré que le contenu des rêves réagit aux événements diurnes d’une manière plus générale et non-spécifique. Le but de notre étude était d’examiner si deux événements qualitativement distincts, un séjour nocturne au laboratoire (LAB), considéré comme un événement interpersonnel, et une tâche de réalité virtuelle (RV), considérée comme un événement non-interpersonnel, sont intégrés de façon différente dans le contenu onirique. Selon nos hypothèses, 1) les éléments spécifiques liés au LAB et à RV seraient incorporés dans les rêves avec des patrons tendances temporels différents, et 2) les incorporations spécifiques seraient associées à des changements plus généraux dans le locus de contrôle (LoC) du rêve. Vingt-six participants ont passé une nuit dans le laboratoire, ont été exposé à une tâche de RV, et ont rempli un journal de rêve pendant 10 jours. Les rapports de rêve ont été cotés pour les éléments spécifiques portant sur LAB et sur RV, et pour l'évolution générale de LoC du rêve. Nos deux hypothèses ont été confirmées: 1) les incorporations de LAB et RV sont négativement corrélées et apparaissent dans le rêve selon des modèles temporels différents. Les incorporations du LAB ont suivi une courbe sinusoïdale en forme de U, avec un effet de résidu diurne et un effet de délai. Les incorporations de RV ont suivi un patron différent, et ont eu un maximum d’incorporations au jour 4. 2) les scores du LoC du rêve étaient plus externes pour le jour 1 (max incorporations du LAB) et plus internes pour le jour 4 (max incorporations de RV). Ces modèles d'incorporation distincts peuvent refléter des différences dans la façon dont les deux événements ont été traités par les processus de consolidation de la mémoire. Dans ce cas, une expérience interpersonnelle (LAB) était incorporée plus tôt dans le temps. Les résultats suggèrent que LoC du rêve reflète les processus de mémoire plus généraux, qui affectent le contenu du rêve entier, et qui sont partiellement indépendants des incorporations spécifiques. / Memories for a daytime event reappear in fragmented form in dream content following a distinct, U-shaped, temporal pattern: immediate incorporations appear on days 1-2 after the event (day-residue effect) and delayed incorporation appear on days 5-7 after the event (dream-lag effect). These two effects are thought to reflect memory consolidation processes linked with dreaming. The U-shaped pattern has been observed for a variety of experimental stimuli, however, no studies have investigated whether dream content also reacts to daytime events in a more general or non-specific way. The aim of this study was to examine whether two qualitatively distinct events, an overnight laboratory (LAB) stay, considered as an interpersonal event, and virtual reality maze task (VR), considered as a non-interpersonal event, are incorporated differently into dream content. We expected that 1) specific elements related to the LAB and VR events would be expressed with different temporal patterns, and 2) these specific incorporations would be associated with more general changes in Dream locus of control (LoC). 26 participants spent one night in the laboratory, underwent a VR maze task, and kept a dream diary for 10 days. Dream reports were scored for specific LAB and VR elements and for general changes in Dream LoC. Two main findings confirmed our expectations: 1) LAB and VR incorporations were inversely related and exhibited distinct temporal patterns. LAB incorporations were U-shaped with both day-residue and dream-lag effects. VR vi incorporations followed a different pattern, with a peak on day 4. 2) Dream LoC scores were more external for day 1 (peak of LAB incorporations) and more internal for day 4 (peak of VR incorporations). These different incorporation patterns may reflect differences in how memory consolidation processes dealt with the two events, with the interpersonal experience being incorporated earlier in time. Dream LoC findings may reflect more general memory processes that are partially independent from the specific incorporations and that affect construction of the whole dream narrative.
304

Cauchemars et mauvais rêves : impact différentiel des méthodes de collecte et analyse descriptive de leur contenu.

Robert, Geneviève 03 1900 (has links)
Cette thèse avait pour objectif général d’approfondir nos connaissances sur les rêves dysphoriques (mauvais rêves et cauchemars), selon deux axes. Premièrement, nous avons voulu éclaircir les implications méthodologiques reliées aux différentes mesures de la fréquence de rappel de rêves (articles 1 et 2). Deuxièmement, nous avons voulu étudier un aspect encore peu exploré des rêves dysphoriques : leur contenu (article 3). Dans la première étude, nous avons comparé systématiquement différentes méthodes rétrospectives et prospectives utilisées pour mesurer la fréquence des cauchemars et des mauvais rêves chez 411 participants. En plus de reproduire les résultats d’études antérieures selon lesquelles les mesure rétrospectives sous-estiment la fréquence des rêves dysphoriques, nous avons démontré que ces fréquences n’étaient pas affectées de manière différentielle par le format de la mesure prospective (journaux de rêves narratifs ou à choix de réponse). Dans la deuxième étude, nous nous sommes intéressés à la fréquence de rappel onirique en général (i.e. de tous les rêves) auprès d'un échantillon de 358 participants pour approfondir les résultats relatifs à la comparaison entre les deux types de journaux de rêves obtenus dans la première étude. Nos résultats soulignent que la fréquence de rappel obtenue par un journal à choix de réponse est plus élevée que celle obtenue d’un journal narratif, et que le présumé effet d’augmentation de rappel attribué aux mesures prospectives est limité dans le temps. Ces résultats suggèrent que des facteurs motivationnels sont impliqués dans la tenue d’un journal de rêves, et que dans le cas des journaux narratifs, ces facteurs outrepasseraient les facteurs attentionnels favorisant le rappel onirique. Dans la troisième étude, nous avons comparé le contenu de 253 cauchemars et 431 mauvais rêves obtenus prospectivement auprès de 331 participants, offrant ainsi l’une des descriptions de ce type des plus détaillées à ce jour. Nos résultats démontrent que cauchemars et mauvais rêves partagent de nombreuses caractéristiques mais se différencient en plusieurs points : le contenu des cauchemars est davantage caractérisé par des menaces physiques, et celui des mauvais rêves par des menaces psychologiques. De plus, les cauchemars ont plus souvent la peur comme émotion principale, ont une intensité émotionnelle plus forte, se terminent plus souvent de façon négative et sont plus bizarres que les mauvais rêves. Ces différences de contenu entre mauvais rêves et cauchemars suggèrent que ces deux types de rêves sont des manifestations d’un même phénomène variant en termes d’intensité, et que cette intensité est multidimensionnelle. Les résultats de l’étude 3 sont discutés en lien avec différentes théories sur l’étiologie et la fonction des rêves. / The overall goal of this thesis was to further our understanding on disturbed dreaming (bad dreams and nightmares) along two main lines of inquiry. First, we examined the methodological implications associated with the different instruments used to measure dream recall frequency (articles 1 and 2). Second, we investigated the actual content of disturbing dreams, a dimension for which empirical data remain surprisingly scarce (article 3). Study 1 compared the frequency of nightmares and bad dreams obtained with retrospective methods and with two types of prospective instruments (narrative and checklist logs) in 411 participants. In addition to replicating findings showing that when compared to daily logs, retrospective self-reports underestimate current nightmare and bad dream frequency, we showed that these frequencies were not differentially affected by the type of prospective log. Our second study extended these findings by comparing the differential effects of narrative and checklist logs on general dream recall in 358 participants. The results indicate that checklist logs yield higher prospective dream recall frequency than narrative logs, and that prospectively measured dream recall frequency tends to peak at the beginning of the log and then remains stable over time. Thus, improved dream recall arising from subjects' increased attention towards their dreams is short-lived and the effect quickly offset by motivational factors. Our third article presents a comprehensive and comparative description of the content of 431 bad dreams and 253 nightmares collected prospectively from 331 participants. The results indicate that although nightmares and bad dreams share many content characteristics and features, they also differ along several dimensions: nightmares tend to involve physical threats whereas psychological threats predominate in bad dreams; nightmares are more likely than bad dreams to contain fear as their principal emotion as well as being significantly more emotionally intense; and when compared to bad dreams, nightmares are more bizarre and contain significantly more aggressions, failures, and unfortunate endings. Taken together these findings support the view that nightmares represent a more intense expression of the same basic phenomenon and that this intensity manifests itself along a number of content dimensions. These results are discussed in relation to different theories on the etiology and function of dreams.
305

Cauchemars et mauvais rêves : impact différentiel des méthodes de collecte et analyse descriptive de leur contenu

Robert, Geneviève 03 1900 (has links)
No description available.
306

Artist as subject : subject as object

Cruise, Wilma 09 1900 (has links)
The artist uses herself as the object of study. Her subjective position is validated within a theoretical framework provided by feminism, existentialism and Freudian theory. The three world views provide the context for an analysis of sculpture produced between the years 1988 and 1997. Three one-person exhibitions held in 1990, 1993 and 1996, are examined in terms of their iconographic emphasis and their theoretical bias. The role of the unconscious in the genesis of the sculptures and the problem of author/reader dichotomies in interpretation are dealt with as thematic threads throughout the dissertation. / Department of History of Art and Fine Arts / M.A. (Fine Arts)
307

Aspectos epidemiol?gicos, cognitivo-comportamentais e neurofisiol?gicos do sonho l?cido

Rolim, Sergio Arthuro Mota 19 June 2012 (has links)
Made available in DSpace on 2014-12-17T15:36:38Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 SergioAMR_TESE_1-152.pdf: 4687573 bytes, checksum: d13791598d6a440077f9a5703901fe3b (MD5) Previous issue date: 2012-06-19 / Coordena??o de Aperfei?oamento de Pessoal de N?vel Superior / Lucid dreaming (LD) is a mental state in which the subject is aware of being dreaming while dreaming. The prevalence of LD among Europeans, North Americans and Asians is quite variable (between 26 and 92%) (Stepansky et al., 1998; Schredl & Erlacher, 2011; Yu, 2008); in Latin Americans it is yet to be investigated. Furthermore, the neural bases of LD remain controversial. Different studies have observed that LD presents power increases in the alpha frequency band (Tyson et al., 1984), in beta oscillations recorded from the parietal cortex (Holzinger et al., 2006) and in gamma rhythm recorded from the frontal cortex (Voss et al., 2009), in comparison with non-lucid dreaming. In this thesis we report epidemiological and neurophysiological investigations of LD. To investigate the epidemiology of LD (Study 1), we developed an online questionnaire about dreams that was answered by 3,427 volunteers. In this sample, 56% were women, 24% were men and 20% did not inform their gender (the median age was 25 years). A total of 76.5% of the subjects reported recalling dreams at least once a week, and about two-thirds of them reported dreaming always in the first person, i.e. when the dreamer observes the dream from within itself, not as another dream character. Dream reports typically depicted actions (93.3%), known people (92.9%), sounds/voices (78.5%), and colored images (76.3%). The oneiric content was related to plans for upcoming days (37.8%), and memories of the previous day (13.8%). Nightmares were characterized by general anxiety/fear (65.5%), feeling of being chased (48.5%), and non-painful unpleasant sensations (47.6%). With regard to LD, 77.2% of the subjects reported having experienced LD at least once in their lifetime (44.9% reported up to 10 episodes ever). LD frequency was weakly correlated with dream recall frequency (r = 0.20, p <0.001) and was higher in men (?2=10.2, p=0.001). The control of LD was rare (29.7%) and inversely correlated with LD duration (r=-0.38, p <0.001), which is usually short: to 48.5% of the subjects, LD takes less than 1 minute. LD occurrence is mainly associated with having sleep without a fixed time to wake up (38.3%), which increases the chance of having REM sleep (REMS). LD is also associated with stress (30.1%), which increases REMS transitions into wakefulness. Overall, the data suggest that dreams and nightmares can be evolutionarily understood as a simulation of the common situations that happen in life, and that are related to our social, psychological and biological integrity. The results also indicate that LD is a relatively common experience (but not recurrent), often elusive and difficult to control, suggesting that LD is an incomplete stationary stage (or phase transition) between REMS and wake state. Moreover, despite the variability of LD prevalence among North Americans, Europeans and Asians, our data from Latin Americans strengthens the notion that LD is a general phenomenon of the human species. To further investigate the neural bases of LD (Study 2), we performed sleep recordings of 32 non-frequent lucid dreamers (sample 1) and 6 frequent lucid dreamers (sample 2). In sample 1, we applied two cognitive-behavioral techniques to induce LD: presleep LD suggestion (n=8) and light pulses applied during REMS (n=8); in a control group we made no attempt to influence dreaming (n=16). The results indicate that it is quite difficult but still possible to induce LD, since we could induce LD in a single subject, using the suggestion technique. EEG signals from this one subject exhibited alpha (7-14 Hz) bursts prior to LD. These bursts were brief (about 3s), without significant change in muscle tone, and independent of the presence of rapid eye movements. No such bursts were observed in the remaining 31 subjects. In addition, LD exhibited significantly higher occipital alpha and right temporo-parietal gamma (30-50 Hz) power, in comparison with non-lucid REMS. In sample 2, LD presented increased frontal high-gamma (50-100 Hz) power on average, in comparison with non-lucid REMS; however, this was not consistent across all subjects, being a clear phenomenon in just one subject. We also observed that four of these volunteers showed an increase in alpha rhythm power over the occipital region, immediately before or during LD. Altogether, our preliminary results suggest that LD presents neurophysiological characteristics that make it different from both waking and the typical REMS. To the extent that the right temporo-parietal and frontal regions are related to the formation of selfconsciousness and body internal image, we suggest that an increased activity in these regions during sleep may be the neurobiological mechanism underlying LD. The alpha rhythm bursts, as well as the alpha power increase over the occipital region, may represent micro-arousals, which facilitate the contact of the brain during sleep with the external environment, favoring the occurrence of LD. This also strengthens the notion that LD is an intermediary state between sleep and wakefulness / O sonho l?cido (SL) ? um estado mental no qual o sujeito est? consciente de estar sonhando durante o sonho. A preval?ncia do SL em Europeus, Norte-Americanos e Asi?ticos ? bastante vari?vel (entre 26 e 92%) (Stepansky et al., 1998; Erlacher & Schredl, 2011; Yu, 2008) e em Latino-Americanos ainda n?o foi investigada. Al?m disso, as bases neurais do SL permanecem controversas. Diferentes estudos observaram um aumento da pot?ncia na frequ?ncia alfa (Tyson et al., 1984), na oscila??o beta na ?rea parietal (Holzinger et al., 2006) e no ritmo gama na regi?o frontal (Voss et al., 2009) durante o SL em rela??o ao n?o l?cido. Assim, para investigar a quest?o epidemiol?gica (Estudo 1), elaboramos um question?rio online sobre sonhos que foi respondido por 3427 volunt?rios. Em nossa amostra, 56% s?o mulheres, 24% s?o homens e 20% n?o responderam o g?nero; a mediana de idade foi de 25 anos. Um total de 76,5% dos indiv?duos refere que lembra dos sonhos pelo menos uma vez por semana. Cerca de dois ter?os dos sujeitos observam o sonho em primeira pessoa, ou seja, vendo o sonho da pr?pria perspectiva e n?o como mais um dos personagens do sonho. Os elementos mais comuns nos sonhos s?o movimentos/a??es (93,3%), pessoas conhecidas (92,9%), sons/vozes (78,5%) e imagens coloridas (76,3%). O conte?do on?rico se relaciona principalmente com planos para o dia seguinte (37,8%) e mem?rias do dia anterior (13,8%). Os pesadelos apresentam principalmente ansiedade/medo (65,5%), ser perseguido (48,5%) e sensa??es desagrad?veis que n?o envolvem dor (47,6%). Assim, sonhos e pesadelos podem ser evolutivamente entendidos como uma simula??o das situa??es frequentes que acontecem na vida e que se relacionam com a nossa integridade social, psicol?gica e biol?gica. Observamos tamb?m que a maioria dos indiv?duos (77,2%) relata ter tido pelo menos um SL, tendo experimentado na sua maior parte at? 10 epis?dios (44,9%). A frequ?ncia do SL foi fracamente correlacionada com a frequ?ncia de lembran?a dos sonhos (r=0,20, p<0,001) e foi tamb?m maior em homens (?2=10,2, p= 0,001). O controle do SL ? raro (29,7%) e inversamente correlacionado com o tempo de dura??o do SL (r=- 0,38, p<0,001), que normalmente ? curto: para 48,5% dos sujeitos o SL dura menos que 1 minuto. A ocorr?ncia do SL ? principalmente facilitada pela possibilidade de dormir sem hora para acordar (38,3%) que aumenta a chance de ter sono REM (SREM), e estresse (30,1%) que aumenta tamb?m as transi??es do SREM para a vig?lia. Como conclus?o, nossos resultados indicam que o SL ? uma experi?ncia relativamente comum (mas n?o recorrente), geralmente fugaz e dif?cil de controlar, o que sugere que o SL ? um est?gio intermedi?rio, incompleto e estacion?rio (ou fase de transi??o) entre o SREM e a vig?lia. Al?m disso, apesar das popula??es Europeias, Norte-Americanas e Asi?ticas terem uma preval?ncia de SL bastante vari?vel, nossos dados de uma amostra de Latino-Americanos fortalecem a no??o de que o SL ? um fen?meno universal da esp?cie humana. Para investigar as bases neurais do SL (Estudo 2), realizamos registros de sono em 32 sujeitos que n?o apresentam SL de forma frequente, e investigamos 6 sujeitos que apresentam SL recorrentemente. A primeira amostra foi submetida a duas t?cnicas cognitivo-comportamentais para induzir o SL: sugest?o pr?- sono (n = 8) e incuba??o de est?mulos do ambiente (pulsos de luz) no sonho durante o SREM (n = 8). Um grupo controle n?o foi submetido a nenhuma das duas t?cnicas (n = 16). Os resultados indicam que ? muito dif?cil induzir SL em laborat?rio, uma vez que conseguimos obter apenas um SL em um sujeito, que era do grupo em que aplicamos a t?cnica de sugest?o pr?-sono. O sinal eletroencefalogr?fico deste volunt?rio apresentou pulsos de ritmo alfa (7-14Hz) anteriores ao SL, de forma breve (aproximadamente 3s), sem altera??o significativa do t?nus muscular e independente da presen?a de movimentos oculares r?pidos. O SL desse sujeito apresentou tamb?m uma maior pot?ncia do ritmo alfa (7-14Hz) na regi?o occipital e um aumento de atividade gama (20- 50Hz) na regi?o temporo-parietal direita. Nos 6 sujeitos que frequentemente t?m SL, o mesmo apresentou em m?dia um aumento de pot?ncia em gama alto (50-100Hz) na regi?o frontal em compara??o com o SREM n?o-l?cido; no entanto, isso aconteceu de forma clara para apenas um dos indiv?duos. Observamos tamb?m que quatro desses volunt?rios apresentaram um aumento da pot?ncia do ritmo alfa na regi?o occipital, pouco antes do SL, ou durante o mesmo. Dessa forma, nossos resultados preliminares sugerem que o SL apresenta diferentes caracter?sticas neurofisiol?gicas dos estados t?picos de SREM e vig?lia: 1) Os pulsos de ritmo alfa, bem como o aumento da pot?ncia dessa oscila??o na regi?o occipital, podem ser micro-despertares. Estes facilitam o contato do c?rebro durante o sono com o meio externo, favorecendo a ocorr?ncia do SL e fortalecendo a ideia de que o SL seria um estado intermedi?rio entre o sono e a vig?lia. 2) Como as regi?es temporoparietal direita e frontal se relacionam com a forma??o da auto-consci?ncia e da imagem corporal, sugerimos que um aumento de atividade nessas regi?es durante o sono pode ser o mecanismo neurobiol?gico subjacente ao SL
308

On Trauma, or, How To Bear Witness to the Quiet Violence of Dreams

Shinners, Keely 01 January 2018 (has links)
This thesis explores South African author K Sello Duiker's The Quiet Violence of Dreams (2001) as a narration of personal and national trauma. This narration of trauma, as a disruption of the past in the present, provides insight to an imagination of recursive temporality. Through the temporal insights trauma introduces, it understands a shared history which is outside of modern, linear progression, a history which is always happening, not needing to prove itself but begging to be witnessed. It is this imagination of a collective, recursive history which translates, in the text, towards a decidedly decolonial witnessing.
309

ASPECTOS ONÍRICOS EM “O HOMEM DA AREIA” DE E.T.A. HOFFMANN

CARIJÓ, SÍLVIA HERKENHOFF 30 October 2017 (has links)
Submitted by Jussara Moore (jussaramoore@id.uff.br) on 2017-10-25T12:46:21Z No. of bitstreams: 1 Dissertação Sílvia Herkenhoff Carijó.pdf: 629346 bytes, checksum: e85c70af7874624bfac6196ba18877e3 (MD5) / Approved for entry into archive by Geisa Drumond (gmdrumond@vm.uff.br) on 2017-10-30T14:30:18Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 Dissertação Sílvia Herkenhoff Carijó.pdf: 629346 bytes, checksum: e85c70af7874624bfac6196ba18877e3 (MD5) / Made available in DSpace on 2017-10-30T14:30:18Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 Dissertação Sílvia Herkenhoff Carijó.pdf: 629346 bytes, checksum: e85c70af7874624bfac6196ba18877e3 (MD5) / Esta dissertação analisa o modo como os sonhos são representados no conto “O homem da areia”, de E.T.A. Hoffmann, e discute algumas de suas funções nessa obra. A existência de uma separação entre um mundo dos sonhos e um mundo da realidade cotidiana na narrativa é indicada, e atributos das experiências relacionadas com esse mundo onírico são apontados, além de formas de acesso a ele. No primeiro capítulo, o autor é apresentado, assim como características de sua obra e a ligação da última com os sonhos. Baseados no trabalho de Pikulik (1987), discutimos aspectos centrais da obra de Hoffmann, como: a divisão entre mundo interior (do maravilhoso) – ao qual os sonhos estão relacionados – e mundo exterior (da realidade cotidiana); as diversas formas como essa divisão é marcada; e a relação que Hoffmann estabelece entre esses mundos. Trabalhamos com uma noção ampliada de sonho, que não os restringe às experiências oníricas que acompanham o sono. Assim, seguimos a argumentação de Heimes (2009), segundo a qual os sonhos, em Hoffmann, estão associados a outros estados em que nos afastamos do consciente – como o delírio ou a loucura –, e parecem se relacionar com algo demoníaco. No segundo capítulo, voltamo-nos para o conto “O homem da areia”, apresentando-o e analisando como os sonhos são representados nele. A figura mitológica do homem da areia está desde sua origem associada aos sonhos e recebe, nesta narrativa, um caráter amedrontador. Para ressaltar os aspectos oníricos da obra, partimos da separação entre o mundo dos sonhos e o mundo do que seria a realidade cotidiana no conto – separação essa que demonstramos estar atrelada à ótica de certos personagens. Logo após, assinalamos alguns elementos que marcam e caracterizam as experiências pertencentes ao mundo dos sonhos no texto, tais como imagens flutuantes e alucinantes; intensidade e fogo; e nuvens, fumaça e vapor. Apontamos também que certos personagens e objetos indicam o momento de transição para esse mundo. Neste trabalho, demonstramos que as experiências oníricas do personagem principal apresentam caráter pressagiador, e que essa característica possui ligação com o destino trágico e inevitável temido por ele. Além disso, a divisão entre os mundos é evidenciada como problemática, por conta da incerteza quanto a que mundo atribuir os acontecimentos narrados. Essa característica de não se poder atribuir ao certo os acontecimentos a um mundo real ou a algo ilusório introduz a narrativa dentro do gênero fantástico, segundo os critérios de Tzvetan Todorov (2007). Também mostramos o lado perigoso do envolvimento do personagem principal com o mundo dos sonhos, uma vez que o leva a se desprender da realidade. Esse desprendimento ocorre gradativamente, havendo inicialmente uma oscilação entre os mundos, culminando na loucura do personagem – que comparamos a um grande pesadelo. Analisamos, neste contexto, a contribuição da revivescência de acontecimentos pressagiados em sonhos anteriores para o enlouquecimento do personagem. Por último, trazemos observações sobre a figuração do homem da areia no conto, mostrando sua ligação com o mundo dos sonhos, assim como sua conexão com a morte. / This dissertation analyzes the ways in which dreams are represented in the short story “The Sandman”, by E.T.A. Hoffman, and discusses some of the functions they perform in it. The existence of a split between a world of dreams and a world of everyday reality is pointed out, and features of the experiences related to the dream world, as well as forms of access to it, are indicated. In the first chapter, I present the author along with some features of his work, including the connection between the latter and dreams. Drawing on the work of Pikulik (1987), I discuss some of the central features of Hoffmann’s work, such as: the division between the inner world (the world of the marvelous) – to which dreams are related – and the outer world (the world of everyday reality); the many ways in which this division is drawn; and the links that Hoffmann establishes between those worlds. I employ a broadened notion of dream – one that is not limited to the dream experiences that accompany sleep. I thus follow Heimes’ (2009) argument that, in Hoffmann, dreams are associated to further mental states in which we slip away from consciousness – such as delusion or madness – and that appear to be related to something demonic. In the second chapter, I turn to the story “The Sandman”: I first present it and then analyze the way dreams are represented in it. The mythical character referred to as Sandman has been from its origin associated to dreams, and in this story it is presented as frightening. In order to highlight the dreamlike features of the story, I draw on the division between the world of dreams and the world of what is portrayed as everyday reality in the story – a division that I show to be linked to the points of view of certain characters. I then point out some of the features that mark and specify those experiences belonging to the world of dreams in the text, such as floating and hallucinatory images; intensity and fire; and clouds, smoke and vapor. I also remark that certain characters and objects indicate moments of transition into that world. I further show that the protagonist’s dream experiences function as omens, and that this feature is linked to the tragic and inevitable fate feared by him. Moreover, the division between the two worlds is shown to be problematic, since it is not clear to which of them the narrated events should be assigned. The fact that the events cannot be unequivocally assigned to the real world or interpreted as illusions makes the story an instance of the fantastic genre, according to Tzvetan Todorov’s (2007) criteria. I also show the dangerous side to the protagonist’s connection to the world of dreams, as it leads him away from reality. Such depart from reality develops gradually, there being initially an oscillation between worlds which later culminates in the protagonist’s madness – which I compare to a vast nightmare. In this connection, I also analyze the part played in the protagonist’s madness by the resurgence of events foreshadowed in dreams. Lastly, I make remarks on the way the Sandman is portrayed in the story, showing his connection both to the world of dreams and to death.
310

Sono e epilepsia: estudo da arquitetura do ciclo vigília-sono em animais do modelo experimental de epilepsia do lobo temporal por pilocarpina. Análise qualitativa e quantitativa / Sleep and epilepsy: study of sleep-awake cycle architecture in animals of pilocarpine model of temporal lobe epilepsy: Qualitative and quantitative analysis

Gabriela de Matos Barbosa Pimenta 02 September 2009 (has links)
INTRODUÇÃO: As relações entre sono e epilepsia são complexas e de grande importância clínica. A melhor compreensão das inúmeras lacunas que permeiam essa relação reforçaria os alicerces para o desenvolvimento de abordagens terapêuticas mais eficazes que pudessem contribuir para o bem-estar do paciente portador de epilepsia e transtornos do sono. OBJETIVO: O presente estudo teve como principal objetivo o estudo comportamental e a caracterização eletrofisiológica do ciclo vigília-sono (CVS) de ratos adultos tornados epilépticos por pilocarpina. MÉTODO: Ratos Wistar machos (N=6), tornados epilépticos após status epilepticus (SE) induzido por pilocarpina e não epilépticos (N=6) foram submetidos à cirurgia extereotáxica para implante de elétrodos bipolares nas áreas corticais (A3, somatosensorial) e hipocampais (CA1) de ambos os hemisférios. Registros contínuos de 24 horas foram submetidos à minuciosa análise visual e os seguintes parâmetros foram analisados: identificação e quantificação dos padrões eletrofisiológicos das fases do ciclo CVS; duração dos episódios oníricos ocorridos durante o sono dessincronizado (SD); padrão de ocorrência do CVS assim como do ciclo de sono (CS), e análise do volume do núcleo supraquiasmático. Os estudos da distribuição do CVS e comportamento onírico foram submetidos à Análise de Variância Multivariada - MANOVA, ao passo que as análises da ocorrência dos ciclos (CVS e CS) e volume do núcleo supraquiasmático foram submetidas ao teste da Análise de Variância (ANOVA) de dois fatores e ao teste de Mann- Whitney, respectivamente. RESULTADOS: Todas as fases do CVS foram identificadas nos ratos epilépticos. As fases da vigília e do sono eram permeadas por espículas e outros grafoelementos epileptiformes, como ondas delta espiculadas no SS e potenciais de alta frequência e baixa voltagem durante VA e o SD. Ao contrário do padrão de ocorrência típico das fases de vigília e sono em ratos não epilépticos, o grupo epiléptico apresentou diferenças significativas quanto à distribuição dessas fases em função do período. Foi observada redução significativa de VA (p<0,002) com concomitante aumento de SS (p<0,005) e vigília relaxada (VR) (p=0,021) no escuro, sendo que a VR era preponderante apenas na primeira metade da noite. Durante o dia, a quantidade de SS era maior no período da manhã (p<0,001), ao passo que houve redução do SD (p=0,002) concomitante com aumento de VA (p<0,001) no período da tarde.Os animais tornados epilépticos por pilocarpina apresentaram redução no padrão de ocorrência do CVS e CS (p=0,004 e p=0,003, respectivamente). Não houve diferença estatística na duração dos episódios oníricos, assim como no volume do núcleo supraquiasmático entre os grupos analisados (p>0,63 e p=0,47, respectivamente). CONCLUSÃO: Os animais epilépticos apresentaram alterações na arquitetura do CVS, bem como nos padrões de ciclicidade evidenciado pelas alterações de comportamento, especialmente no ciclo escuro. Esses fatos sugerem possível comprometimento estrutural e/ou funcional das circuitarias responsáveis pela geração e manutenção das fases de vigília e sono, assim como dos sistemas de temporização do CVS. Tomados em conjunto, os dados reproduziram anormalidades do CVS observadas em pacientes epilépticos, sugerindo que o presente modelo pode ser uma importante ferramenta para o estudo de mecanismos subjacentes à epilepsia do lobo temporal e sono. / INTRODUCTION: Relationships between sleep and epilepsy are complex and have great clinical importance as well. The full understanding of the various gaps present in this relationship would pave the ground for new studies that could generate new clinical approaches aiming to contribute to the well-being of the patient suffering from epilepsy and sleep disorders. OBJECTIVE: The present study aimed to carry out a behavioral analysis and electro-oscillographic characterization of the phases of sleep-wake cycle (SWC) of pilocarpine- induced epilepsy in adult rats. METHODS: Male Wistar rats that became epileptic after 60 days of pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus (SE) (N=6) and non epileptic ones (N=6) were submitted to extereotaxic surgery for implantation of bipolar electrodes in cortical (A3, somestesic) and hippocampal (CA1) areas in both hemispheres. Twenty-four hour continuous registers were submitted to detailed visual analysis and the following parameters were studied: identification and quantification of electrophysiological parameters of phases of SWC, duration of oniric episodes during desynchronized sleep (DS), the pattern of occurrence of SWC and cycles of sleep (CS). In addition, the volume of suprachiasmatic nuclei was investigated. To analyze the architecture of sleep-wake phases and oniric behavior, Multivariate Analysis of Variance-MANOVA was utilized, whereas the pattern of cycles (SWC and CS) and volume of suprachiasmatic were submitted to Analysis of Variance with 2 factors-Two-way ANOVA and Mann-Whitney test, respectively. RESULTS: In the epileptic rats all phases of SWC were identified. The phases of wake and sleep were permeated by spikes and graph elements epileptiforms such as spiked delta waves in SS and low frequency waves with high voltage during AW and SD phases. In contrast to the pattern of normal rhythmic activity evident in non-epileptic rats the epileptic group presented significant differences concerning distribution of the phases of SWC according to the period. In the dark cycle significant reduction of AW (p<0.002) was observed concomitantly with an increase of SS (p<0.005), while the relaxed wakefulness (RW) showed an increase during the first half of the night (p=0.021). In the light cycle, the SS was more prominent in the morning period (p<0.001), following by a reduction of DS (p=0.002) concomitantly with an increase of AW (p<0.001) during the afternoon in the epileptic group. The number of cycles with a regular sequence of each phase from awake to sleep (SWC) was significantly decreased (p=0.004), as was the number of cycles of sleep (p=0.003) in epileptic rats. No significant differences were found in duration of oniric episodes and volume of suprachiasmastic nuclei (p>0.63 e p=0.47, respectively) between non epileptic and epileptic groups. CONCLUSION: The data obtained revealed that after SE the epileptic animals presented some alterations in the SWC architecture as well as in the cyclicity patterns mainly in dark cycle. Such facts suggest a possible functional and/or structural impairment in the circuitry responsible for the generation of sleep and wake phases and in the SWC timing system. Taken together the data reproduced the abnormalities observed in patients, suggesting that the pilocarpine model is a suitable one to study sleep dysfunctions in temporal lobe epilepsy.

Page generated in 0.1231 seconds